This episode I discuss our sense of pain and pleasure: where and how
they each arise in our mind and body and various ways to control their
intensity. I discuss the science of behavioral tools like acupuncture
and hypnosis and directed pressure, including the neural circuits they
each activate to modulate our experience of pain or pleasure. I also
discuss whole body pain, pain "syndromes" and novel pain relief
compounds such as Acetyl-L-Carnitine, SAMe and Agmatine. I discuss
neuroplasticity of the pain system and the key role that visual
perception plays in pain modulation. Finally, I address the link
between dopamine, serotonin, and oxytocin, with arousal, pleasure and
pain. As always, both basic science and various protocols are
described.
- Skin, Pain, Pleasure
- Protocol 1: Maximizing Motivation (with Dopamine & Pleasure)
- Sponsors: InsideTracker, Helix Sleep, Athletic Greens
- Pleasure & Pain, & Skin Sensors
- Sensing Touch with Your Brain: Magnification of Feet, Hands, Lips, Face, Genitals
- Two-Point Discrimination, Dermatomes
- Thoughts & Genes That Make Physical Pain Worse
- Expectations, Anxiety, & Pain Threshold
- Protocol 2: Cold Sensing Is Relative; Getting Into Cold Water
- Protocol 3: Heat Is Absolute
- Injury & Pain
- Protocol 4: Plasticity of Pain: Key Role of Vision
- Sensing Disparate Body Parts As Merged
- Pain “Syndromes”, Psychogenic Fever, “Psychosomatics”
- Fibromyalgia, Naltrexone, Protocol 5: Acetyl-L-Carnitine
- Protocol 6: Agmatine, S-adenosyl-L-methionine (SAMe), L-5-Methyltetrahydrofolate
- Acupuncture: Mechanism, Non-Responders, Itch & Inflammation
- Laser Photobiomodulation, Protocol 7: Hypnosis (reveri.com)
- Protocol 8: Pressure-Based Pain Relief, “Gate Theory of Pain (Relief)”
- Redheads & Pain Thresholds, Endogenous Opioids
- Protocol 8: Love & Pain, Dopamine
- Pleasure & Reproduction, Dopamine & Serotonin, Oxytocin
- Protocol 9: PEA, L-Phenylalanine (Precursor to Tyrosine)
- Contextual Control of Pleasure by Autonomic Arousal, Dopamine Baselines
- Pleasure-Pain Balance
- Protocol 10: Controlling Pleasure, Dopamine & Motivation Over Time
- Protocol 11: Immediate, Non-Goal-Directed Pleasure, PAG
- Direction of Touch: Pleasure Versus Pain, Arousal & Touch “Sensitivity”
- Synthesis & How to Conceptualize Pain and Pleasure, Support
- hubermanlab #pain #pleasure #dopamine #motivation
- [Andrew Huberman] Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we
discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. [bright
upbeat music] -- I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a Professor of
Neurobiology and Ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today,
we continue our discussion of the senses. And the senses we are going
to discuss are pain and pleasure. Pain and pleasure reflect two
opposite ends of a continuum, a continuum that involves detection of
things in our skin and the perception, the understanding of what those
events are. Our skin is our largest sensory organ and our largest
organ indeed. It is much larger than any of the other organs in our
body. And it's an odd organ if you think about it, it has so many
functions. It acts as a barrier between our organs and the outside
world, it harbors neurons nerve cells that allow us to detect things
like light touch, or temperature or pressure of various kinds. And
it's an organ that we hang ornaments on. People put earrings in their
ears. People decorate their skin with tattoos and inks and other
things. And it's an organ that allows us to experience either great
pain or great pleasure. So it's a multifaceted organ and it's one that
our brain needs to make sense of in a multifaceted way. So today we're
going to discuss all that. And most importantly, how you can
experience more pleasure and less pain by understanding these
pathways. We will also discuss things you can do, and if you wish
things you can take that will allow you to experience more pleasure
and less pain in response to a variety of different experiences.
Before I go any further, I want to highlight a particularly exciting
area of science that relates to the skin and to sensing of pleasure
and pain, but has everything to do with motivation. Motivation is
something that many people struggle with, not everybody, but most
people experience dips and peaks in their motivation even if they
really want something. How should we think about these changes in
motivation? What do they reflect? Well at a very basic level, they
reflect fluctuations changes in the levels of a chemical called
dopamine. Most of us have heard of dopamine. Dopamine is a
neuromodulator meaning it modulates or changes the way that neurons
nerve cells work. Most of us have heard that dopamine is the molecule
of pleasure. However, that is incorrect. Dopamine is a molecule of
motivation and anticipation. To illustrate how dopamine works, I want
to highlight some very important work largely carried out by the
laboratory of a guy named Wolfram Schultz. The Schultz Laboratory has
done dozens of excellent experiments on the dopamine system and have
identified something called reward prediction error. Although in some
sense you can think about it as reward prediction variance. Changes in
the levels of dopamine depending on whether or not you expect a reward
and whether or not you get the reward. So I'm going to make this very
simple. Dopamine is released into the brain and body and generally
makes us feel activated and motivated, as if we have energy to pursue
a goal. And it is released into the brain and body in anticipation of
a reward. Measurements of dopamine have been made in animals and
humans. And what you find is that when we anticipate a reward,
dopamine is released, we will put in the work to achieve that reward.
That work could be mental work or physical work, but when the reward
arrives, dopamine levels drop back down to baseline. That's right.
When we receive a reward, dopamine levels go back down to baseline. So
the way to envision this as you can just imagine a sort of increase in
dopamine as we anticipate something we're working towards it, we're
working towards a goal. We're excited about seeing somebody or meeting
somebody or receiving some reward and then the reward comes and
dopamine goes down. Now that's all fine and good, but there is a way
to get much more dopamine out of that process and therefore a way to
have much more motivation, energy, and focus because those are the
consequences of elevated dopamine. The way to do that is to not
deliver the reward on an expected schedule. So experiments have been
done where there's an anticipation of a reward, there's work, and then
the reward only arrives every other or every third about of work,
okay? So this would be like getting a pat on the head. If you're a dog
or a, perhaps a child or an adult, or getting a monetary reward only
for every third project or every third race that you win. Pick any
kind of goal. It doesn't matter. These molecules don't care about what
you're pursuing. They are a common currency of different types of
activities. That's a regular reward schedule, and it will not alter
the pattern of dopamine release that I described before. However, if
the reward arrives intermittently almost randomly, so you anticipate a
reward as a maybe it might come, it might come. Then you work, work,
work, work, work, no reward. You repeat the work, work, work, work,
work, work, and then you get a reward. So some trials, you do some
trials, you don't, and it's completely random. Under those conditions,
the amplitude, the amount of dopamine that's released into your system
and the motivation to continue working hard or playing whatever kind
of game you're playing, doubles or triples. And this is the basis of
things like slot machines and gambling. And this is why so many people
will give so much of their money up to casinos and the casinos always
win. Sometimes people walk away with more money than they came to the
casino with, but the vast majority of the time, the house wins as they
say. And it's because they understand intermittent reward schedules.
And you can apply this to stay motivated in your own pursuits. Rather
than thinking about the pleasure of a reward, understand that dopamine
is released in response to anticipation reward, and that is the fuel
for work. And every once in awhile at random remove the reward. That's
the way to continue to stay motivated. Not to reward every action or
every goal. And this is also true, if you're trying to train up
children or train up players on a team, you should not celebrate every
win. I know that's a little counter-intuitive, we're going to go more
into the biology of dopamine and how it relates to the pleasure system
later on in the podcast, but for now understand intermittent reward
schedules harness the biology of dopamine in ways that can allow you
essentially infinite motivation over time.
Before I go any further, I want to acknowledge that this podcast is
separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is
however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer
information about science and science-related tools to the general
public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of
today's podcast. Our first sponsor is InsideTracker. InsideTracker is
a personalized nutrition platform that analyzes data from your blood
and DNA to help you better understand your body and help you reach
your health goals. I've long been a believer in getting regular blood
work done for the simple reason that many, if not all of the factors
that impact your immediate and long-term health can only be analyzed
from a quality blood test. And nowadays with the advent of DNA tests,
you can get further knowledge into the mechanisms that are going on in
your body and how those mechanisms are changing as they relate to
metabolism and hormones and other aspects that profoundly impact your
wellbeing. The problem with most blood tests, however, is that you get
information back and it will tell you whether or not your levels of
something are higher, low, or within range, but it doesn't give you
any directives. It doesn't tell you what to do with that information.
With InsideTracker has this wonderful feature. It's a very easy to use
dashboard that has suggestions about lifestyle factors, exercise,
nutrition, supplementation, et cetera, that one could take in order to
adjust those numbers according to your personal goals. So the whole
thing is made very easy. In fact, they can come to your home to do the
blood test and DNA test if you like. If you'd like to try
InsideTracker, you can go to insidetracker.com/huberman. And if you do
that you'll get 25% off any of InsideTracker's plans. Just use the
code huberman@checkout. And I should also mention that through August
15th, 2021, you can get a free inner age test. That's a measure of
your biological age according to your DNA and other factors with any
purchase from InsideTracker store. So go to insidetracker.com/huberman
to get 25% off any of InsideTracker's plans and up to up and through
August 15th, you can get a free inner age test. Today's episode is
also brought to us by Helix Sleep. Helix makes mattresses and pillows
that are ideally suited to your sleep needs. Everybody's different.
Some people sleep on their sides, some people sleep on their stomach,
some people sleep on their back, some people run warm during the
night, some people run cold. If you go to the Helix's website,
helixsleep.com, you can take a quick two-minute quiz, and then it will
tell you what your ideal mattress is. It will match you to your ideal
mattress. I did this and I matched to the so-called Dusk mattress. I
like a mattress that's not too firm nor too soft. I tend to run hot
during the night and I tend to sleep on my side or on my stomach. I
started sleeping on a Helix mattress about nine months ago, and I'm
sleeping better than I ever have before. So if you're interested in
upgrading your mattress to one that's ideally suited to your sleep
needs, go to helixsleep.com/huberman, take their two-minutes sleep
quiz, and they'll match you to a customized mattress. And you'll get
up to $200 off that mattress order, and two free pillows. They have a
10-year warranty and you get to try out the mattress for a 100 nights
risk-free, they'll even pick it up for you if you don't love it, but I
think you really will love it. So again, if you're interested, go to
helixsleep.com/huberman for up to $200 off and two free pillows.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Athletic Greens. Athletic
Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral probiotic drink. I started
drinking Athletic Greens way back in 2012. And so I'm delighted that
they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started drinking Athletic
Greens and the reason why I still drink it once or even twice a day is
because it takes care of all my foundational nutritional needs. It
includes vitamins, minerals, and probiotics which are important for
the gut microbiome, this colony of bacteria, healthy bacteria that we
all have living in our guts. And when that bacteria is healthy, our
brain functions better, our immune system functions better. There is
now a lot of research to support that. Athletic Greens taste
delicious. I mix mine with water and a little bit of lemon juice. And
as I mentioned, I drink it once or twice a day, usually in the morning
and again in the afternoon. If you'd like to try Athletic Greens, you
can go to athleticgreens.com/huberman and claim a special offer. That
special offer is a year supply of vitamin D3K2. There's now a robust
literature showing that vitamin D3 is critical for various aspects of
our health, metabolic health, endocrine health, et cetera. And you'll
also get five free travel packs. When you travel mixing up Athletic
Greens is made really easy by these little travel packs. You just put
them in a jar or other a water bottle, mix it with water. It's very
easy to do on a plane or in the et cetera. Again, go to
athleticgreens.com/huberman to get the Athletic Greens, the five free
travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3K2. So let's talk about
pleasure and pain.
I think we all intuitively understand what pleasure and pain are.
Pleasure generally is a sensation in the body and in the mind that
leads us to pursue more of whatever is bringing about that sensation.
And pain is also a sensation in the body and in the mind that in
general leads us to want to withdraw or move away from some activity
or interaction. That's not always the case. Some people actively seek
out pain. Some people somehow can't seem to engage with or experience
pleasure, but most people operate on this basis of pleasure and pain.
Scientists would call this appetitive behaviors, meaning behaviors
that lead us to create an appetite for more of those behaviors and
aversive behaviors, behaviors that make us want to move away from
something. The simplest example of that would be putting your hand
near a hot flame, at some point, there would be a reflex or a deep
desire to withdraw your hand. Tasting something delicious in general
makes us want to eat more of that thing. Interactions with other
people that we find delicious, also make us want to interact with
those people more. None of this is complicated or sophisticated. This
is simply to illustrate the fact that pleasure and pain tend to evoke
opposite responses, opposite behavioral responses and opposite
emotional responses. So how does that come about? Well, it really
comes about by an interaction that starts at one end of our body,
meaning our skin and the other end of the organs of our body, which is
deep within the brain. So let's consider these two ends of the
spectrum of pleasure and pain and what they contribute to those
experiences of pleasure and pain. The organ that we call the skin, as
I mentioned earlier, is the largest organ in our body. And throughout
that organ, we have neurons, little nerve cells. Now to be really
technical about it, and the way I'd like you to understand it is that
the so-called cell body meaning the location of a cell in which the
DNA and other goodies the kind of central factory of the cell, that
actually sits right outside your spinal cord. So all up and down your
spinal cord on either side are these little blobs of neurons, little
collections of neurons. They have a name if you'd like to know for you
aficionado or those who are curious, they're called DRGs dorsal root
ganglia. A ganglion is just a collection or clump of cells. And those
DRGs are really interesting because they send one branch that we call
an axon, a little wire out to our skin, also to our muscles into our
organs, but here we're talking about the skin. They send a wire out to
our skin and that wire literally reaches up into the skin. It's
actually in our skin and they have another wire from that same cell
body that goes in the opposite direction, which is up to our brain and
creates connections within our brain in the so-called brainstem. What
this means is that the neuron in your body that we call the DRG that
sends a wire an axon to sense what's going on in your big toe and then
sends another axon in the opposite direction into the base of your
brain, that is the largest cell in your entire body of any kind. Fat
cell, muscle cell, nerve cell, et cetera. Is extremely long cells. It
can be a meter or more depending on how tall you happen to be. So we
have these cells that have wires they go off in two different
directions and the wire that's within our skin will respond to any
number of different categories of stimuli. These wires are positioned
within the skin to respond to mechanical forces. So maybe light touch,
some will only send electrical activity up toward the brain in
response to light touch. Meaning if you press on the skin really hard,
they don't respond. You stroke the skin lightly with your fingertips
or a feather and they respond very robustly. Others respond to course
pressure, the hard pressure, but they won't respond to a light
feather, for instance. Others respond to temperature. So they will
respond to the presence of heat or the presence of cold or changes in
heat and cold. And still others respond to other types of stimuli,
like certain chemicals on our skin. Many of you have probably
experienced the sensation of eating a hot pepper. Well, I don't
recommend doing this, but were you to take a little slice of jalapeno
or other hot pepper, habanero pepper or something like that and rub it
on your skin, you would actually feel something at that location. And
that's because that pepper doesn't just create a sensation within your
mouth, it will create a similar sensation on your skin. So these
neurons are amazing. They're collecting information of particular
kinds from the skin throughout the entire body and sending that
information up toward the brain. And what's really incredible, I just
want you to ponder this for a second. What's really incredible is that
the language that those neurons use is exactly the same. The neuron
that responds to light touch sends electrical signals up toward the
brain, the neurons that respond to cold or to heat or to habanero
pepper, they only respond to the particular thing that evokes the
electrical response. I should say that they only respond to the
particular stimulus, the pepper, the cold, the heat, et cetera, that
will evoke an electrical signal, but the electrical signals are a
common language that all neurons use. And yet, if something cold is
presented to your skin, like an ice cube, even if you don't see that
ice cube, if your eyes are closed, or someone comes up behind you and
puts an ice cube against your bare skin back, you know that that
sensation, that thing is cold. You don't misperceive it as heat or as
a habanero pepper. So that's amazing. What that means is that there
must be another element in the equation of what creates pleasure or
pain.
And that element is your brain. Your brain takes these electrical
signals and interprets them. Partially based on experience, but also
there are some innate meaning some hard wired aspects of pain and
pleasure sensing that require no experience whatsoever. A child
doesn't have to fall down but once to know on that first fall, that
hurt. They don't have to touch a flame, but once and the very first
time they will withdraw their hand from the flame. So no prior
experiences required. Other things prior experience is required. For
instance, if you're somebody that has an intense, intense aversion to
spicy foods, that's probably because you've tasted spicy foods before.
Likewise, if you really like sweet foods, it's probably because you've
tasted them before. So you can start to make predictions based on
prior experience, but the pain and pleasure system don't need prior
experience. What they need is a brain that can interpret these
electrical signals that can take these electrical signals and somehow
create what we call pleasure and pain out of them. So what parts of
the brain? Well, mainly it's the so-called somatosensory cortex. The
portion of our neocortex, which is on the outside of our brain, that
kind of bumpy part, not kind of if you have a normally formed brain,
it will be bumpy. If you have a smooth brain that's not good. Some
animals just have a smooth brain. Humans have a bumpy brain, which
means it has a very large surface area. And those bumps are 'cause you
squeezed it like a pizza and clump and you bunched it all up and put
inside the skull. That's good. That means you have a lot of neurons.
And in your somatosensory cortex, you have a map of your entire body
surface. That map is called a homunculus. And if we were to take your
cortex and lay it out on a table, I've actually done this with the
cortices of various animals and humans included. What you would find
is that there's literally a map of your entire body surface. But it
wouldn't look exactly like you, this map would be very distorted. Why
would it be distorted? Well, certain areas of your body have a much
denser innervation as we call it, or put simply many more of these
sensory wires from these DRGs within your skin. So this map of you
that exists in your brain, and you do have one of these on each side
of your brain, so you have two of these maps to homunculi, that is
you, it's your representation of touch including pleasure and pain.
And in that map, your lips are enormous. And your back is very, very
small. And the area around your eyes and the area representing your
face is absolutely enormous. So you would look like some sort of odd,
weird clay doll from some sort of bizarre late night animation thing.
And just imagine the psychedelic experience of that character of you
and that's what it would look like. But it's not randomly organized.
To the contrary, it's highly organized in a very particular way, which
is that the areas of your skin that have the highest density of the
sensory receptors are magnified in your brain. So it's sort of like
having more pixels in a certain part of a camera than others, and in
doing that allowing higher resolution in this case of touch, not a
vision, but of touch sensation in certain parts of your body. What are
the areas that are magnified? Well, the lips, the face, the tips of
the fingers, the feet, and the genitals. And so this map of you has
very large lips, face, tips of fingers, bottoms of feet, and genitals.
And that's because the innervation, the number of wires that go into
those regions of your body far exceeds the number of wires for
sensation of touch that go to other areas of your body. You can
actually experience this in real time right now by doing a simple
experiment that we call two-point discrimination.
Two-points discrimination is your ability to know whether or not two
points of pressure are far apart, near each other, or you actually
could perceive incorrectly as one point of pressure. You might want a
second person to do this experiment. Here's how you would do it. You
will close your eyes, that person would take two fine points. Don't
make them too sharp, please. So it could be two pencils or pens or the
backs of pens. Two pens I'm holding in my hands. If you're just
listening to this, I'm just holding two pens. My favorite pens, these
pilot, V5 or V7, which I love. If you were to close your eyes and I
were to take these two pens and put their points close together about
a centimeter apart and present them to the top of your hand, I'm going
to just going to do that now to myself. You, even though your eyes
were closed, you would be able to perceive that that was two points of
pressure presented simultaneously to the top of your hand. However, if
I were to do this to the middle of your back, you would not experience
them as two points of pressure. You would experience them as one
single point of pressure. In other words, your two point
discrimination is better, is higher on areas of your body which have
many, many more sensory receptors. You are more sensitive at those
locations. Now this makes perfect sense once you experience it or you
hear about it. However, most of us don't really appreciate how
important and what a profound influence this change in density of
receptors across our body surface has. And we can go a step further
and describe another feature of the way that you're built and the way
that you experience pleasure and pain, which is called the dermatome.
The dermatome is literally the way in which your body surface is
carved up into different territories. Much like a map of the United
States is carved up into different territories of states and counties,
et cetera. The dermatome is the way in which neurons connect to
different parts of your body. Now, you've actually experienced the
dermatome before. The dermatome is when you have a neuron that
connects to a particular area of the body and that neuron doesn't just
send one little wire out like one little line and go up into the skin
to detect mechanical, or thermal, or chemical stimuli. It actually
sends many branches out like a tree. But remember those branches of
the tree come from one single neuron. Now, occasionally what will
happen is you will experience something like cold, or heat, or pain,
or tingling on a patch of your body. And occasionally that patch of
body will actually have a very cleanly demarcated boundary, a very
stark boundary with the areas around it. A good example of this would
be the herpes simplex 1 virus, which if one has this virus, and I
should mention that somewhere between 80 and 90% of people have this
virus, this is not a sexually transmitted virus. This is a virus is
transmitted very easily between people through various forms of
contact non-sexual contact, it's present in children, it's present in
adults and most people get it, some get symptoms and some don't some
get recurring symptoms, some don't. We can talk about that at the end,
if you like, but this virus lives on what's called the fifth cranial
nerve also called the trigeminal nerve. The trigeminal nerve sends
branches out to the lips, to the eyes, and to certain portions of the
face. So for those of you listening, I've just kind of put my right
hand across my face and to sort of simulate the three branches, the
trigeminal aspect of this nerve. So tri three. Now, when the herpes
virus flares up, as they say, in response to stress or other factors,
the virus inflames that nerve and people experience tingling and pain
on the nerve. Sometimes they'll get a cold sore or a blister on their
lip or near their mouth, sometimes they'll get a collection of those.
And that's because that dermatome is actually inflamed. Now other
people will experience something like shingles. It's a fairly common
viral infection. And what they'll notice is they'll get a rash that
has a boundary. It's like, they'll get a bunch of bumps, sometimes
blisters, and it'll have a sharp boundary. That boundary exists
because the virus exists on the nerve. And so it actually is
boundaried with the neighboring area of the body that's receiving
input from another nerve and that one doesn't have the virus living on
it. So anytime you see a rash or a pattern on the body surface on the
skin that has a pretty stark boundary, chances are that's an event
that's impacting the dermatome. I've experienced this before then not
through herpes simplex, but through the experience of having a lot of
blood sort of aggregating in a kind of a segment across the front of
my face, it was really bizarre. I looked in the mirror and I thought,
what is going on here? I was having an allergic reaction to something
I'd eaten. And that allergic reaction clearly was affecting one of the
nerves and therefore the dermatome and what it showed up was, it was
almost like someone had drawn lines on my face that said, okay, this
rash or this reaction rather can happen here, but not in a region
right next to it. Whenever you see that chances are it's a reaction of
the nerves of the dermatome. So you'll start to see these things more
and more when you start to look for them. You don't always have to
have a viral infection to experience this. Sometimes you'll just
experience tingling or even a pleasant sensation, and it will be
restricted in kind of a strict boundary on one location or your body
surface and not another. Not corresponding to an organ like, okay,
this arm or just your feet or something like that. But just a segment.
It's almost like someone outlined a particular area of her body
surface. That's the dermatome. So you've got sensors in the skin and
you've got a brain that's going to interpret what's going on with
those sensors.
In fact, we can take an example of a sudden rash or inflammation at
one location, the dermatome, and we can ask what would make it hurt?
What would make it worse? What would make it go away? And believe it
or not, your subjective interpretation of what's happening has a
profound influence on your experience of pleasure or pain. There are
several things that can impact these experiences, but the main
categories are expectation. So sort of whether or not you thought or
could expect that this thing was going to happen, right? If someone
tells you this is going to hurt, I'm going to give you an injection
right here, it might hurt for a second. That's very different and your
experience of that pain will be very different than if it happened
suddenly out of the blue. There's also anxiety, how anxious, or how
high or low your level of arousal, autonomic arousal, that's going to
impact your experience of pleasure or pain. How well you slept and
where you are in the so-called circadian or 24-hour cycle. Our ability
to tolerate pain changes dramatically across the 24-hour cycle. And as
you can imagine is during the daylight waking hours that we are better
able to tolerate, we are more resilient to pain, and we are better
able to experience pleasure. At night our threshold for pain is much
lower. In other words, the amount of mechanical or chemical or thermal
meaning temperature stimulated that can evoke a pain response and how
we rate that response is much lower at night. And in particular, in
the hours between 2:00 AM and 5:00 AM, if you're on a kind of standard
circadian schedule. And then the last one is our genes. Pain threshold
and how long a pain response lasts is in part dictated by our genes.
And later I'm going to discuss this myth or whether or not it's really
a myth as to whether or not certain people in particular red heads,
people who have reg pigmented hair and fair skin, whether or not their
pain thresholds differ. And to just give you a little sneak peek into
that, indeed they do and it's because of a genetic difference in a
particular gene, in a particular pattern of receptors in the skin that
are related to the pigmentation of hair and skin. So we have
expectation, anxiety, how well we've slept, where we are in the so-
called 24-hour circadian time and our genes. So let's talk about
expectation and anxiety because those two factors can powerfully
modulate our experience of both pleasure and pain in ways that will
allow us to dial up pleasure if we like and to dial down pain, if
indeed that's what we want to do. So let's talk about expectation and
anxiety because those two things are somewhat tethered. There are now
a number of solid experiments, both in animal models and in humans
that point to the fact that if we know a painful stimulus is coming,
that we can better prepare for it mentally and therefore buffer or
reduce the pain response. However, the timing in which that
anticipation occurs is vital for this to happen. And if that timing
isn't quite right, it actually can make the experience of pain far
worse. So here I'm summarizing a large amount of literature, but
essentially if subjects are warned that a painful stimulus is coming,
their subjective experience of that pain is vastly reduced. However,
if they are warned just two seconds before that pain arrives, it does
not help. It actually makes it worse. And the reason is they can't do
anything mentally to prepare for it in that brief two second window.
Similarly, if they are warned about pain that's coming two minutes
before a painful stimulus is coming electric shock or a poke or cold
stimulus or heat stimulus that's pretty extreme, that also makes it
worse because their expectation ramps up the autonomic arousal. The
level of alertness is all funneled toward that negative experience
that's coming. So how soon before a painful stimulus should we know
about it if the goal is to reduce our level of pain? And the answer is
somewhere between 20 seconds and 40 seconds is about right. Now, I'm
averaging across a number of different studies, but if you have about
20 seconds or 40 seconds advance warning that something bad is coming,
you can prepare yourself for that, but the preparation itself and the
arousal that comes with it, the kind of leaning in, okay, I'm either
going to relax myself or I'm going to really kind of dig my heels in
and kind of meet the pain head on. That seems to be the optimal
window. This can come and useful in a variety of contexts, but I think
it's important because what it illustrates is that it absolutely
cannot be just the pattern of signals that are arriving from the skin,
from these DRGs, these neurons that connect to skin that dictates our
experience of pain or pleasure. There has to be a subjective
interpretation component, and indeed that's the case. So let's talk
about the range of pain experiences.
And from that, we will understand better what the range of pleasure
experiences are that different people have because we are all
different in terms of our pain threshold. First of all, what is pain
threshold? Pain threshold has two dimensions. The first dimension is
the amount of mechanical or chemical or thermal stimulation that it
takes for you or me or somebody else to say, I can't take that
anymore. I'm done. But there's another element as well which is how
long the pain persists. I'll just describe myself for example, I don't
consider myself somebody who has a particularly high pain threshold. I
don't think it's particularly low either, but I wouldn't consider
myself somebody that has a particularly high pain threshold. When I
stubbed my toe against the corner of the bed, it absolutely hurts. But
one thing that I've noticed is that I have very sharp inflections,
very high inflections in my perception of pain, and then they go away
quickly. I don't know if that's adaptive or not. It's probably not,
but my experience of pain is very intense, but very brief. Other
people experience pain in a much kind of slower rising, but longer-
lasting manner. And to just really point out how varied we all are in
terms of our experience of pain, let's look to an experiment. There
been experiments done at Stanford School of Medicine and elsewhere,
which involved having subjects put their hand into a very cold vat of
water and measuring the amount of time that they kept their hand in
that water. And then they would tell the experimenter very quietly,
how painful that particular stimulus was on a scale of one to 10 so-
called Likert scale for your aficionados. That simple experiment
revealed that people experience the same thermal in this case, cold
stimulus, vastly different. Some people would rate it as a 10 out of
10 extreme pain. Other people would rate it as barely painful at all,
like a one. Other people, a three other people, a five, et cetera. Now
what's interesting is that the same thing is true for experience of a
hot, painful stimulus 120 degree hot plate, where you have to put your
hand on it, and then at some point you remove your hand. Some people
are able to keep their hand on there the whole time, but people rate
that experience as very painful, a little bit painful or moderately
painful depending on who they are. Now, that's interesting, probably
not that surprising, however, but what is very interesting is that
when the same experiment was done on medical doctors or medical
doctors in training, they too of course experienced pain through a
range of subjective experiences. Some of them just like any other
person off the street said a particular stimulus of a particular
temperature was very painful, other said it wasn't painful at all and
some said it was moderately painful. And that turns out to be vitally
important for the treatment of pain because pain is not an event in
the skin. Pain is a subjective, emotional experience. You may have
heard that we have a particular category of these DRGs that innervate
the skin, which are called nociceptors. Nociceptor comes from the word
noci, nocere I believe it is, which means to harm. However,
nociceptors don't carry information about pain, they carry information
about particular types of stimuli impacting the skin. And then the
brain assigns a value of valence to it, a label and says that's
painful. And where people draw the line between not painful and
painful varies. Now, because physicians are people and because
physicians treat pain, what we know from a lot of data now is that if
someone comes into the clinic and says they're experiencing chronic
pain or whole body pain or acute pain after an injury or one location,
it doesn't really matter what the cause is or even if there's a cause
at all, how the doctor reacts to that report of the patient's pain
will dictate in many cases the course of treatment. And of course
doctors, their goal is to treat the patient to going to the patient's
needs, not their own. And that's what good doctors do. However, it's
been found and I think now there is work being done to try and change
this, but if a doctor has a very high threshold for pain, their
interpretation of somebody else's report of pain is going to be
different. They might not discount the patient, right? This doesn't
necessarily mean that they think, oh, this person their pain is
irrelevant, probably not. In fact, from having a high threshold for
pain, if someone comes in and says, I'm in extreme pain, that doctor
probably thinks, wow, this has to be really, really extreme, but they
can be talking about two different experiences. Similarly, if a
physician has a very low threshold for pain, and someone comes in and
says, yeah, I'm experiencing some pain in my back. I've got the
sciatica thing, but yeah, it's a little bit uncomfortable. It's like
a, I don't know like a four out of 10. Well, that physician might
interpret that four out of 10 as a pretty extreme sense of pain or
pretty extreme experience of pain. And so you can start to see how the
subjective nature of pain can start to have real impact on the
treatment of pain because treatment of pain is carried out by
physicians. In fact, there is no objective measure of pain. We can ask
how long somebody can keep their hand on a hot plate or in a cold
bath. You can do various experiments. They even have some extreme
experiments where they'll shave a portion of the leg and they'll put
on a very painful chemical compound and see how long people can
tolerate that. These are very uncomfortable experiments as you can
imagine, but in general, we don't have a way of measuring somebody
else's subjective experience of pain. There is no blood pressure
measure. There's no heart rate beats per minute measure of pain. So
one of the great efforts of neuroscience and of medicine is to try and
come up with more objective measures of pain. Similarly, pleasure is
something that we all talk about. Ooh, that feels so good, or I love
that or more of that please or less of that, but we have no way of
gauging what other people are experiencing except what they report
through language. And so this is really just to illustrate that this
whole thing around pain isn't a black box. We do have an understanding
of the elements. There are elements in the skin, there's elements of
the brain, there's expectation, anxiety, sleep, and genes, but that it
is very complicated. And yet there are certain principles that fall
out of that complicated picture that can allow us to better understand
and navigate this axis that we call the pleasure-pain axis. So rather
than focus on just the subjective nature of pain, let's talk about the
absolute qualities of pain and the absolute qualities of pleasure so
that we can learn how to navigate those two experiences in ways that
serve us each better.
First of all, I want to talk about heat and cold. We do indeed have
sensors in our skin that respond to heat and cold. And for any of you
that have entered a cold shower or a cold body of water of any kind or
ice bath, et cetera, you will realize that getting into cold is much
harder if you do it slowly. Now, despite that people tend to do it
very slowly. I have noticed an enormous variation with which people
can embrace the experience of cold. I noticed it because I do some
work with athletes and I do some work with military and I do some work
with the general public. And one of the best tests of how somebody can
handle pain is to ask them to just get into an ice bath. It's not a
very sophisticated experience, but it really gets into the core of the
kind of circuitry that we're talking about, both in the skin and in
the brain. Some people regardless of sex, regardless of age, and
regardless of physical ability can just get into the cold. They're
somehow able to do it. Now, I don't know what their experience of the
cold is. And neither do you. You only know your experience, but
they're able to do that. Some do it quickly, some do it slowly. Others
find the experience of cold to be so aversive that they somehow cannot
get themselves in. They start quaking, they start complaining and many
of them just simply get out. They can't do it. Some don't even get in
past their knees. This isn't necessarily about pain threshold, but
it's related to that. I think it can be helpful to everyone to know
that even though it feels better at a mental level to get into the
cold slowly and people ask, oh, I just want to get in slowly, I want
to take my time. It is actually much worse from a neuro-biological
perspective. The neurons that sense cold respond to what are called
relative drops in temperature. So it's not about the absolute
temperature of the water. It's about the relative change in
temperature. So as you move from a particular temperature, whether or
not it's in the air next to an ice bath or cold shower, or from a body
of water that's warm to a body of water that's colder, or sometimes in
the ocean, you'll notice it's warm. And then as you swim out further,
you'll get into a pocket of water where it's much colder. That's when
the cold receptors in your skin start firing and sending signals up to
your brain. Therefore, you can bypass these signals going up to the
brain with each relative change one degree change, two degrees change,
et cetera, by simply getting in all at once. In fact it is true and
maybe you've been told this before, and it is true that if you get
into cold water up to your neck, it's actually much more comfortable
than if you're halfway in and halfway out. And that's because of the
difference in the signals that are being sent from the cold receptors
on your upper torso, which is out of the water in your lower torso.
Now, I wouldn't want anyone to take this to mean that they should just
jump into an unknown body of water. There are all sorts of factors
like currents, and if it's very, very cold, yes, indeed. You can stop
the heart. People can have heart attacks from getting into extremely
cold water, like a melted mountain stream that's been frozen all
winter, or has been very, very cold or as a snow pack going into it.
If very cold, you can indeed have a heart attack. So please be smart
about how cold and what bodies of cold water you happen to put
yourself into. But it is absolutely true that provided it safe,
getting into a cold water is always going to be easier to do quickly
and is going to be easier to do up to your neck. In fact, you actually
want to get your shoulders submerged. There are a number of other
things you can do if you really want and it's safe to do. You can put
your face under and activate the so-called dive reflex, which also
makes the tolerance of cold easier believe it or not. So it's very
counterintuitive. It's like getting into water faster and more
completely you will experience as less uncomfortable, less cold. And
indeed that's the case. And that's because these cold receptors are
measuring every relative drop in temperature. So every single one is
graded as we say in biology, it's not absolute. As an additional
point, if you're sitting in a body of cold water and it's not
circulating, you'll notice that you start to warm up a little bit. Or
even if you feel like you're freezing cold, if you move and that water
around you moves, of course, then you'll notice it's got even colder.
And that's because there's a thermal layer. You're actually heating up
the water that surrounds your body like a halo around every aspect of
your body. A sort of silhouette of you of heat where you're heating
that water. When you move, you disrupt that thermal layer. Now heat is
the opposite.
Heat and the heat receptors in your skin respond to absolute changes
in temperature. And this is probably because our body and our brain
can tolerate drops in temperature much better than it can tolerate
increases in temperature safely. So when you move from say a standard
outdoor environment, I mean, here in the States we measure in terms of
Fahrenheit. So maybe it's a 75 or an 80 degree, or even 90 degree day,
and you get into a 100 degrees sauna, or if you're in a cool air
conditioned building and you go outside and it's very warm outside,
you sort of feel like the heat hits you all at once, boom. Hits you
all at once, kind of like a slap in the face, but then it will just
stay at that level. Your body will acclimate to that particular
temperature. However, if that temperature is very, very high, you'll
notice that your experience of that heat and your experience of kind
of pain and discomfort and your desire to get out of that heat will
tend to persist. You don't really adapt in the same way. And certain
people who are really good at handling very hot sauna, get better at
this. You learned to calm your breathing, et cetera, lower your
autonomic arousal. Obviously you don't want to let your body
temperature go too high because neurons cook, they die. If neurons
die, they don't come back and that's bad. Many people unfortunately
harm themselves with hyperthermia. Everyone has a different threshold
for this, but in general, you don't want your body temperature to go
up too high. That's why a fever of like 103 starts to become worrisome
104. You really get concerned if and it goes up into that range or
higher, that's when you need to really cool down the body or get to
hospital so they can cool you down. Heat is measured in absolute terms
by the neurons. So gradually moving into heat makes sense, and finding
that threshold, which is safe and comfortable for you, or if it's
uncomfortable, at least resides within that realm of safety. So that's
heat and cold, and those are sort of non-negotiables. You can try and
lower your level of arousal. In fact, many people who get into a cold
shower and ice bath I think the recommendation that I always give is
that you have two possible approaches to that. You can either try and
relax yourself, kind of just stay calm within the cold, or you can
lean into it. You can actually take mental steps to generate more
adrenaline to kind of meet the demands of that cold. And at some point
we'll do a whole episode on how to use cold and heat to certain
advantages we've done a little bit of this in past episodes using the
cold to supercharge human performance and things of that sort. But in
general, cold is measured in relative terms. And therefore getting in
all at once is a good idea provided you can do it safely. And heat is
measured in absolute levels by your brain and body and therefore you
want to actually move into it gradually. So it's the kind of the
inverse of what you might think.
One of the most important things to understand about the experience of
pain and to really illustrate just how subjective pain really is, is
that our experience of pain and the degree of damage to our body are
not always correlated. And in fact, sometimes can be in opposite
directions. A good example of this would be x-rays. We all
occasionally get x-rays at least in the US we get x-rays when we go to
the dentist from time to time, and the occasional x-ray might be safe
depending on who you are provide you're not pregnant, et cetera. I've
gone to the dentist. They put you in the chair, they cover you with
the lead blanket, and then they run behind the screen to protect
themselves. And they beam me with the x-rays to get a picture of your
teeth and your jaws and your skull, et cetera. Well, if you were to
get too many x-rays, you could severely damage the tissues of your
body, but you don't experience any pain during the x-ray itself. In
contrast, you can think that your body is damaged and experienced
extreme pain and yet your body can have no damage. A classic example
of this was published in the British Journal of Medicine, in which a
construction worker fell from I think it was a second story, which he
was working and a nail went up and through his boot and he looked down
and he saw the nail going through his boot and he was in absolute
excruciating pain. They took him to the hospital and because the nail
was so long and because of where it had entered, it exited the boot,
they had to cut away the boot in order to get to the nail. And when
they did that, they revealed that the nail had passed between two of
his toes. It had actually failed to impale his body in any way. And
yet the view, the perception of that nail entering his boot at one end
and exiting the boot at the other was sufficient to create the
experience of a nail that had gone through his foot. And the moment he
realized that that nail had not gone through his foot, the pain
completely evaporated. And this has been demonstrated numerous times.
People that work in emergency rooms actually see variations on this.
Not always that extreme, but many times what we see and how we
perceive that wound or that event has a profound influence on how we
experience pain. And I mentioned this, not just because it's a kind of
sensational and fantastic example of this extreme, subjective nature
of pain, but also because it brings us back to this element which is,
we don't know how other people feel. Not just about pain, but about
pleasure. We think we do, we have some general sense of whether or not
an event ought to be painful or pleasurable, but actually we barely
understand how we feel let alone how other people feel, and we can be
badly wrong about how we feel meaning we can misinterpret our own
sense of pain or our own sense of pleasure depending on what we see
with our eyes and what we hear with our ears. So we hear a scream like
a shrill scream, and we think it must be pain. And if we look at
something that's happening to somebody and it fits a prior category or
a prior representation of what we would consider painful stimulus,
well, then we think that they're an extreme pain, but actually they
might not be in pain at all. Now this highly subjective nature of pain
and the way in which we use our visual system to interpret other
people's pain and our own pain has actually been leveraged to treat a
very extreme form of chronic pain. And it's an absolutely fascinating
area of biology and neuroscience. And it's one that we can actually
all leverage toward reducing our own levels of pain whenever we are
injured. Or believe it or not, even in chronic pain.
To describe this area of science requires a kind of extreme example,
but I want to be clear that even if you don't suffer from this extreme
example, there's relevance and a tool to extract for you. The extreme
example is that of an amputated digit, meaning one of your fingers or
your toes, or of an amputated limb. So people that have digits or
limbs that are gone missing from an injury or surgical removal, will
often have the experience that it's still there. The so-called phantom
limb phenomenon. Now, why would that be? Well, when you remove a
particular finger or limb, obviously that finger and limb is gone and
the dorsal root ganglion neuron that would normally send a wire out to
that particular region of the body, that wire is no longer there
because that portion of the body is no longer there. And in some
cases, those neurons die almost always, but not always. However, the
map your so-called homunculus, your representation of yourself in the
brain is still there. And this map, the so-called homunculus map that
you have in that I have is very plastic. It can change. And so as a
consequence areas of the map that adjacent to one another can actually
start to invade other areas of the map. So for instance, there are
neuroimaging studies that have documented that somebody that has say a
complete removal of their left arm, the representation of their left
arm still exists in the cortex. And experimentally if one is to
stimulate that area of the cortex, that person, and if that person
were you, would experience having that arm that it were being
stimulated, even though it's not there. Now, someone who has an
amputated arm doesn't need to have their brain stimulated in order to
have the experience of that phantom limb being present. In fact, many
people who have limbs that were amputated feel as if that limb is
still present even though obviously it's not. And no matter how many
times they look to the stump and just see a stump, somehow it doesn't
reorganize that homunculus so-called central brain map. Now, that
would be fine. You might even think that would be better, better to
think you have the arm there than to feel as if it's missing, and yet
many people who have amputated limbs report phantom limb pain. They
don't feel that the arm is just casually draped next to them. They
feel as if it's bunched up and it's an extreme pain. In fact, this
kind of contorted stance that I'm taking right here in my chair is not
unlike the way that these patients described this. They feel as if
it's kind of cramped up, it's very uncomfortable for them. Now, and
absolutely creative and you could even say genius scientists by the
name of Ramachandra. That's actually his last name, his complete name
is a little bit more complicated. So you all almost always hear
Ramachandran referred to as Ramachandran or V.S Ramachandran because
his full name is Vilayanur Subramanian Ramachandran. So a lot of
letters in there, a lot of vowels, but Ramachandran is a
neuroscientist. He was actually a colleague of mine when my lab was
formerly at the University of California, San Diego. Has done a lot of
work on this phantom limb phenomenon. And Ramachandra actually started
off as a vision scientist. And he understood the power of the visual
system in dictating our experience of things like pain and pleasure.
And so what he developed was a very low technology yet
neuroscientifically sophisticated treatment for phantom limb. It
consisted of a box, literally a box that had mirrors inside of it. And
the patient would put the intact hand or limb into one side, and
obviously they couldn't put the amputated limb into the other side,
but because of the configuration of the mirrors, it appeared as though
they had two symmetric limbs inside the box. And then he would have
them look at that limb and move it around. And as they would do this,
they would report real time movement, or I should say real time
perception of movement in the phantom limb. Now this is absolutely
incredible but makes total sense when you think about the so-called
top down or contextual modulation of our sensory experience, remember
it's anticipation, it's anxiety, it's interpretation of what's
happening that drives our perception of what's happening. And so, as
he would have these patients move their intact limb to a more relaxed
position, the patients would feel as if the phantom limb were
relaxing. And this was used successfully to treat phantom limb pain in
a number of different people. It didn't always work. And you can
imagine sometimes it might be a little trickier like for a leg
although there have been leg boxes that have been developed and
arranged for this purpose. And what was remarkable is that they could
finish these experiments and have the patient, the person enter a
state of relaxation, reduced the pain in the phantom limb, and it
would stay there even though, of course, as they exited the mirror
box, they would go about their life and use their intact limb for its
various purposes. I love this experiment because it really speaks to
the subjective nature of pain and pleasure. It speaks to the power of
the visual system, like what we see just like the nail through the
boot experiment. What we see profoundly impacts our experience of
pleasure and pain in this case pain. Now, there's another aspect to
the phantom limb experience and of these maps, the so-called
homunculus maps in the cortex that Ramachandran worked on, which has
very interesting and reveals the degree to which these maps are
plastic or can change in response to experience. Turns out that
because of the locations of different body part representations within
these maps, certain parts of our body that normally we don't think of
as related can start to create merged experiences.
What do I mean by that? Well, Ramchandran described a patient who had
a somewhat odd experience of having lost their foot. So they actually
had their foot amputated about midway up the Achilles. So lower
portion of the calf and foot. I don't recall what the reason was for
having it removed. And fortunately for this patient, they did not
experience pain in that portion of their body, but rather they
confided in him that whenever they would have sex, they would
experience their orgasm in their phantom foot in addition to in their
genitals, of course. And Ramachandra understood the homunculus map.
And he understood that this was because the representation of the foot
within the homunculus actually lies adjacent to, and is somewhat
interdigitated with it actually kind of merges with the representation
of the genitalia. Now that's a weird situation. And yet you now know
that the density of innovation of the feet and the genitalia, as well
as the lips and the face are actually the highest sensory innervation
that you have in your entire body. And this speaks to, I think, a more
important general principle for all people of the experience of
pleasure or pain, which is that an aspect of our pain or pleasure can
be highly localized. It can be because of a cut to a particular
location on the body or it can because of a fall injury or a kind of
bruise on one side of our body. And yet our experience of pleasure and
pain can also be an almost a body-wide experience. And yet it's always
most rich. It's always most heightened in these regions of our body
that have dense sensory innervation. So we experience pain and
pleasure according to local phenomenon receptors in the skin, and this
homunculus map that has all these different territories, but because
of the way that those territories are related, this kind of wild
example of somebody experiencing orgasm in their phantom foot speaks
to the larger experience. The more typical rather experience that I
should say that all people have, which is that pleasure can be body-
wide or we can experience it in our face or the bottoms of our feet
and other areas of the body that we experience pleasure and similarly
with pain. And that brings us to the topic of whole body pain, not
just localized pain, as well as whole body pleasure not just localized
pleasure.
There are a number of examples of whole body pain that people suffer
from. And one common one is called fibromyalgia. I want to just first
share with you a little bit of medical insight. A few months back, I
did an Instagram live with Dr. Sean Mackey who's an MD medical doctor
and a PhD at Stanford School of Medicine that was recorded and placed
on my Instagram. If you want to check it out, we can provide a link to
that in the show notes. Dr. Mackey is the Chief of the Division of
Pain at Stanford School of Medicine. So he's a scientist. He studies
pain and he treats patients dealing with various forms of pain, whole
body pain, like fibromyalgia, acute pain, et cetera. And he shared
with me something very interesting, which is that anytime you hear or
see the word syndrome, that means the medical establishment does not
understand what's going on. A syndrome is a constellation of symptoms
that point in a particular direction or some general set of directions
about what could be going on, but it doesn't reveal a true underlying
disease necessarily. It could be aggregative diseases or it could be
something else entirely. And I want to make sure that I emphasize the
so-called psychosomatic phenomenon. I think sometimes we hear
psychosomatic and we interpret that as meaning all in one's head. But
I think it's important to remember that everything is neural, whether
that's pain in your body 'cause you have a gaping wound and you're
hemorrhaging out of that wound or whether or not it's pain for which
you cannot explain it on the basis of any kind of injury. It's all
neural. So saying body, brain, or psychosomatic it's kind of
irrelevant. And I hope someday we move past that language.
Psychosomatic is interesting. There was a paper that was published in
2015, and then again in 2020 a different paper focused on the so-
called psychogenic fevers or psychosomatic effects. I just briefly
want to mention this because it relates back to pain. These studies
have shown that there are areas of the so-called thalamus, which
integrates and filter sensory information of different kinds. And
within the brainstem, an area called the DMH and I can also provide a
link to this study if you like, that shows that there is a true
neurological basis. There are brain areas and circuits that are
related to what's called psychogenic fever when we are stressed. And
in particular, if we think that we were injured or that we were
infected by something, we can actually generate a true fever. It is
not an imagined fever. It is our thinking generating an increase in
body temperature. And so this has been called psychosomatic. It's been
called psychogenic, but it has a neural basis. So when we hear
syndrome and a patient comes into a clinic and says that they suffer
for instance, from something which is very controversial frankly like
chronic fatigue syndrome, some physicians believe that it reflects a
real underlying medical condition, others don't. However, syndrome
means we don't understand. And that doesn't mean something doesn't
exist. Fibromyalgia or whole body pain for a long time was written off
or kind of explained away by physicians and scientists frankly, my
community as one of these syndromes. It couldn't be explained.
However, now there is what I would consider and I think others would
and should consider from understanding of at least one of the bases
for this whole body pain.
And that's activation of a particular cell type called glial. And
there's a receptor on these glial for those of you that want to know
called the toll 4 receptor and activation of the toll 4 receptor is
related to certain forms of whole body pain and fibromyalgia. Now,
what treatments exist for fibromyalgia. And even if you don't suffer
from fibromyalgia, and even if you don't know anyone who does this is
important information because what I'm about to tell you relate to how
you and your body, which is you of course can deal with pain of any
kind. And there are actually things that one can do and take that can
encourage nerve health in general. In other conditions like diabetic
neuropathy, but in all individuals. So there are clinical data using a
prescription drug. This is work that actually was done by Dr. Mackey
and colleagues. The drug is called naltrexone. Naltrexone is actually
used for the treatment of various opioid addictions and things of that
sort, but it turns out that a very low dose, I believe it was a one
10th the size of the typical dose of naltrexone has been shown to have
some success in dealing with and treating certain forms of
fibromyalgia. And it has that success because of its ability to bind
to and block these toll four receptors on glial. So this so-called
syndrome or this thing that previously was called a syndrome,
fibromyalgia actually has a biological basis. It was not just
inpatients heads. And I really tip my hat to the medical establishment
including Dr. Mackey and others who explored the potential underlying
biologies of things like fibromyalgia and are starting to arrive at
treatments. Now, I'm not a physician, I'm a professor, so I'm not
prescribing anything. You should talk to your doctor of course, if you
have fibromyalgia or other forms of chronic or whole body pain to
explore whether or not these low dose naltrexone treatments are right
for you. But I think it's a beautiful case study if you will, not a
case study of an individual patient, but a case in study of linking up
the patient's self-report of these experiences and using science to
try to establish clinical treatments. There's another treatment, or I
should say there's another approach that one could take. And again,
I'm not recommending people do this necessarily. You have to determine
what's right and say for you, I cannot do that. There's no way your
situation's very far too much, and it would be outside of my
wheelhouse to prescribe anything, but there's a particular compound
which in the United States is sold over the counter and in Europe is
prescription. It's one that I've talked about on this podcast before
for other purposes. And that compound is acetylcarnitine.
Acetylcarnitine as I mentioned is by prescription in most countries in
Europe and the US you can buy this over the counter. There is evidence
that acetylcarnitine can reduce the symptoms of chronic whole body
pain and other certain forms of acute pain at dosages of somewhere
between one to three and sometimes four grams per day. Now
acetylcarnitine can be taken orally. It's found in 500 milligram
capsules, as well as by injection. By injection in the States in the
United States that is also requires a prescription or requires a
prescription I should say. The over-the-counter forms are generally
capsules or powders. Those apparently do not require a prescription.
There are several studies exploring acetylcarnitine in this context,
as well as for diabetic neuropathy. And what's interesting about
acetylcarnitine is it's one of the few compounds that isn't just used
for the treatment of pain, but has also been shown in certain contacts
to improve peripheral nerve health generally. And for that reason,
it's an interesting compound. I've also talked about acetylcarnitine
on here previously, because it has robust effects on things like spur
motility and health, including the speeds at which sperms swim, how
straight they swim turns out that swimming for sperm is more efficient
if they swim straight, as opposed to like those kids in on the swim
team, they're like banging up against the lane lines and zigzagging
all over the place. So it does turn out to be the case that the
quickest route between any two places is a straight line and the good
sperm know that, and the less good sperm don't seem to know that. And
acetylcarnitine seems to facilitate straight swimming trajectories as
well as speed of swimming and overall sperm health. And there is
evidence from quality peer reviewed studies showing that
acetylcarnitine supplementation can also be beneficial for women's
fertility in ways that it affects perhaps we don't really know the
mechanism, health and status of the egg or egg implantation. There are
a large number of studies on acetylcarnitine. You can look those up on
Pub Med, if you like, or on examine.com. There are some studies that I
don't think are included there which are particularly interesting. One
that I just would like to reference the last name of the first author
is Mahdavi M-A-H-D-A-V-I. The title of the paper is Effects of l
-Carnitine Supplementation on Serum Inflammatory Markers and Matrix
Metalloproteinases Enzymes in Females with Knee Osteoarthritis. So
this is a randomized double blind placebo controlled pilot study that
showed really interesting effects of short term supplementation of
acetylcarnitine. Longer term, the effects were less impressive. So
it's pretty interesting that this compound has so many different
effects. How could it have these effects? Well, it appears that it's
having these effects through its impact on the so-called inflammatory
cytokines. Inflammatory cytokines for those of you that don't know are
secreted by the immune system in response to different stressors,
physical stressors, mental stressors too food that you eat that isn't
good for you. The so-called hidden sugars. Yes, will increase
inflammation if they're ingested too often, or in amounts that are too
high in quantity. Things like Interleukin 1 beta, things like
C-reactive protein, things like interleukin 6. Interleukin 6 is kind
of the generic inflammatory marker that all studies refer to. And yet
there are other interleukins, please note that there are other
interleukins like interleukin 10 that are anti-inflammatory. So immune
system can secrete inflammatory molecules to deal with wounds and
stress and things. And in the short term that's good, and in the
longterm that's bad. And it can secrete anti-inflammatory cytokines
like IL10. And these matrix metalloproteinases, it's kind of a
mouthful, but these matrix metalloproteinases are very interesting.
Anytime you see A-S-E, ASE that's generally an enzyme, which means
that these compounds in this case, these matrix metalloproteinases are
used to break down certain elements around wounds and scoring which
might sound like a bad thing, but in some cases is good because it
allows certain cells like glial cells so-called microglia to come in
like low ambulances, like low paramedics and clean up wounds. So
scarring and inflammation is kind of a double-edged sword. It can be
good, but too much scarring. If it contains a wound too much, doesn't
allow the infiltration of cell types to move in and take care of that
wound and heal it up. So it appears that L-carnitine is impacting a
number of different processes, both to impact pain and perhaps, and I
want to underscore perhaps, but there are good studies happening now,
perhaps accelerate wound healing as well.
As long as we're talking about acute pain and chronic pain and
supplementation and non-prescription drugs, at least in the United
States that people can take to deal with pain of various kinds, I'd be
remiss if I didn't mention the two that I get asked most often about,
which are agmatine and S-adenosyl-L-methionine, which is sometimes
called SAMe. Both of those have been shown to have some impact
categorized on examine as notable impact on various forms of pain, due
to osteoarthritis, or due to injury of various kinds of indifferent
subject population, men, women, people of different ages, et cetera.
SAMe in particular has been interesting because it's been shown head
to head with drugs like Naproxen and other drugs of that sort, which
are well established and sold over the counter in the US to work at
least as well as some of those compounds at certain doses. But it's
also shown that SAMe and some of those things take more time in order
to have those effects. In fact, head to head with things like Neproxin
have been shown that they can take up to a month in order to have the
pain relieving effect. Now, whether or not that makes them a better
choice or a worst choice really depends on your circumstances. I'm
certainly not recommending that anybody take anything, but I do think
it's interesting and important to point out that things like agmatine,
things like SAMe have been shown under certain circumstances to be
beneficial for pain and they are outside the realm of prescription
drugs. I think this is a growing area of some people call them
supplements, some people call them nutraceuticals. Look, at the end of
the day these are compounds that affect cellular processes. And the
more that we understand how they affect those cellar processes as we
now do for things like acetylcarnitine, I think the more trust that we
can put into them, or the more to which we might want to avoid them
because of some of the side effects or contra-indications that those
compounds could have. If you're interested in those other compounds, I
do invite you as I always do to check out examine.com, but also to do
your research on those compounds by simply putting them into Google or
putting them into PubMed, which would be even better. And if you are
going to go into PubMed, if you're going to start playing scientist,
which I do encourage you to do, I would encourage you to not just read
abstracts, but if you can, if the studies are freely available, I
realized not all of them are freely available to try and read those
studies at least to the extent that you can. There's a particularly
nice study that you might look at that was published in 2010 in Pain
Medicine, which is Keynan et al K-E-Y-N-A-N, which looked at the
safety and efficacy of dietary agmatine sulfate on lumbar disc
associated radiculopathy not laughing at the condition. It's a painful
condition that describes a, it's a kind of a range of symptoms that
relate to pinching of nerves. The spinal columns, I was laughing at my
pronunciation of it. That particular study is quite good. And the
conclusion of that study that they drew was that there were limited
side effects and that dietary agmatine sulfate is safe and efficacious
for treating and alleviating pain and improving quality of life and
lumbar-disc associated pain. However, there were very specific dosage
regimens that were described there and duration of treatment. And so
you should not take anything that I say or that study to mean that you
can just take this stuff willy-nilly or at any concentration of
course, or dose. You always want to pay attention to what the science
says. That paper fortunately is freely available online. And we will
also provide a link to that study. For those of you that are
interested in SAMe and its usage for the treatment of various types of
pain, and perhaps other benefits, a number of companies have stopped
making SAMe instead what they're now focusing on is what they think is
a better or more bioavailable alternative, which is
5-methyltetrahydrofolate or 5-MTHF. This molecule is necessary for
converting homocysteine to methionine, which is then converted to SAMe
so rather than taking SAMe directly, the idea is to take something
that's upstream of SAMe and make more SAMe endogenously available.
This is a different strategy. I've talked about this strategy before
for increasing other things like growth hormone, et cetera. There's
always this question of whether or not in trying to increase the
amount of a particular molecule in the body, whether or not taking
that specific molecule was the best thing or working further upstream
as it's referred to working on the precursor or increasing the levels
of the precursor is the better way to go. It appears that this 5-MTHF
is the strategy that people are now taking in place of taking SAMe
directly. So in other words, they're taking this in order to get
elevated levels of SAMe. Now, I'd like to turn our attention to a
completely non drug, non supplement related approach to dealing with
pain.
And it's one that has existed for thousands of years. And that only
recently has the Western scientific community started to pay serious
attention to, but they have started to pay serious attention to it.
And there is terrific mechanistic science to now explain how and why
acupuncture can work very well for the treatment of certain forms of
pain. Now, first off, I want to tell you what was told to me by our
director or Chief of the Pain Division at Stanford School of Medicine,
Dr. Sean Mackey, which was that some people respond very well to
acupuncture and others do not. And the challenge is identifying who'll
respond well and who won't respond well. Now, when I say won't respond
well, that doesn't necessarily mean that they responded in a negative
way, that it was bad for them, but it does appear that a fraction of
people experienced tremendous pain relief from acupuncture and others
experience none at all or very little to the point where they have to
seek out other forms of treatment. The science on this is still
ongoing. There was actually an excellent paper published on this in
the Journal of the American Medical Association, one of the premier
medical clinical journals. And it basically reinforced the idea that
you have responders and non-responders. A number of laboratories have
started to explore how acupuncture works. And one of the premier
laboratories for this is Qiufu Ma's lab at Harvard Medical School.
Qiufu has spent many years studying the pain system and a system
that's related to the pain system, which is the system that controls
our sensation of itch. Just as a brief aside about itch, itch and pain
are often co associated with one another. I was recently in Texas, and
I will tell you, they have some mean mosquitoes. They're small, but
whatever they're injecting into your skin. Well, here I am talking now
about my subjective experience of pain, whatever they injected into my
skin felt to me like the most extreme mosquito bites I've ever had,
not while they were biting me, not while they were injecting the
venom, but boy, those Texas mosquitoes make me itch. How do they do
it? Well, their venom creates little packets of so-called histamine
that travel around those packets are called mast cells, little packets
of histamine that go to that location and make me and presumably you
want to scratch those mosquito bites. I scratch mine you scratch
yours, but we both scratch our mosquito bites. And when we do that,
the histamines are released. That gets red and inflamed and the itch
even worse. The inflammation is actually caused by the histamine.
Well, that experience of inflammation and pain and itch is what we
call a pyrogenic experience. So we we have pain which is nociception,
essentially, I know that the pain of aficionado always get a little
upset because they say, oh, there's no such thing as as a pain
receptor, it's no susceptive receptors and pain is subjective
experience. Yes, I acknowledge all that. But for fluency, let's just
think about pain as a certain experience and itch as a separate
experience, but they often exist together because those mosquito bites
were what I would call painful, or at least not pleasant. They didn't
just itch, they were also painful nuts because itch brings with it
inflammation and inflammation often brings with it pain relief, but it
can also bring with it the sensation of pain. So itch and pain are two
separate phenomenon. It was actually discovered through a really
interesting phenomenon that relates to something that is actually
consumed in supplement form, which is this tropical legume. It's
actually a bean called Mucuna pruriens. That's M-U-C-U-N-A that's one
word P-R-U-I-E-N-S Mucuna pruriens is a bean, it's as legume that this
bean is 99% L-DOPA. It's dopamine or rather it's the precursor to
dopamine and people buy this stuff and take it over the counter as
ways to increase their levels of dopamine. It does make you feel
really dopamine doubt, meaning it makes you feel a little high and
really motivated and really energetic a lot like other drugs that will
do that. I don't necessarily recommend taking Mucuna pruriens. I
personally don't like taking it. Doesn't make me feel good. I crash
really hard when I take it. But on the outside of this bean is a
compound that makes people itch. So they remove this when you take it
in supplement form. In fact, it's usually in capsule form, but the
outside of this bean, it's like a hairy bean. And those little hairs
contain a compound, which was actually used to study and identify
these itchy receptors in the skin. So we don't have time to go into
all the details of itch, but it's pretty interesting that you have
these compounds out in nature that can make us itch. Inside them they
have dopamine. I mean, this is really weird, but plant compounds are
really powerful. So don't let anyone tell you that because something's
from a plant or an earth that it's not powerful. There are very
powerful plant and herb compounds. Mucuna prurien being one of them
with dopamine on the inside and itchy stuff on the outside. Now, what
does this all have to do with acupuncture? Well, Qiufu Ma's lab has
not just identified the itch pathway, this pruritus genes as they're
called, which causes itch and the pyrogenic phenomenon of itch being
separate from pain. His lab has also studied how acupuncture causes
relief of, but also can exacerbate pain. Now, the form of acupuncture
that they explored was one that's commonly in use called
electroacupuncture. So this isn't just putting little needles into
different parts of the body. These needles are able to pass an
electrical current, not magically, but because they have a little wire
going back to a device and you can pass electrical current. Here's
what they found. This is a study published in the journal neuron Cell
Press journal, excellent journal, very high stringency. So what Qiufu
Ma's lab found was that if electroacupuncture is provided to the
abdomen, to the stomach area, it creates activation of what are called
the sympathetic ganglia. These have nothing to do with sympathy in the
emotional sense has to do with the stress response. Sympa just means
together. So it activated a bunch of neurons along the spinal cord.
And the activation of these neurons evolves neural adrenaline and
something called NPY neuropeptide Y. The long and short of it is that
stimulating the abdomen with electroacupuncture was either anti-
inflammatory or it could cause inflammation. It could actually
exacerbate inflammation depending on whether or not it was of low or
high intensity. Now that makes it a very precarious technique. And
this may speak to some of the reason why some people report relief
from acupuncture and others do not. However, they went a step further
and stimulated other areas of the body using electroacupuncture. And
what they found is that stimulation of the legs of the hind limbs, as
it's called an animals, and the legs in humans caused a circuit, a
neural circuit to be activated that goes from the legs up to an area
of the base of the brain called the DMV not the DMH, which I mentioned
earlier, but the DMV like you go to the DMV, which is a miserable
experience for most people, forgive me, DMV employees, but let's be
honest most people don't enjoy going to the DMV as patrons, but we
have to so we go. The DMV and low intensity stimulation, this
electroacupuncture of the hind limbs activated the DMV and activated
the adrenal glands, which sit at top of your kidneys and cause the
release of what are called catecholamines. And those were strongly
anti-inflammatory. In other words, electroacupuncture of the legs and
feet can, if done correctly, be anti-inflammatory and reduce symptoms
of pain. And can we think accelerate wound healing because activations
of these catecholaminergic pathways can accelerate wound healing as
well. So the takeaway from this is that while there are thousands of
years and millions of subjects involved in explorations of
electroacupuncture and acupuncture, Western medicine is starting to
come into this and start to explore underlying mechanism. Now, for
those of you that love acupuncture and are real proponents of it, it's
worked for you, you might say, well, why does Western medicine even
need to come into this? Why should they even be exploring this? But we
should all be relieved that they are because what's starting to happen
now is that as the mechanistic basis for this is starting to come to
light, insurance coverage of things like acupuncture is starting to
emerge as well. And this is in contrast to other therapies for which
there's a lot of anecdotal evidence, but very little mechanistic
understanding. One example of that would be laser photo biomodulation
the use of lasers of different types really to treat pain and to
accelerate wound healing. A lot of people claim that this can really
help them. However, most places, at least in the States, won't cover
this with insurance or don't perform this in standard clinics. And the
reason is the underlying mechanism isn't known. I'm not going to get
into the argument about whether or not mechanistic understanding
should or should not be required in order to have insurance coverage
of things that work. That's not what this is about. And that actually
will be a boring discussion because I'm shouting at a tunnel through
you. And I wouldn't be able to hear you shout back no matter what your
stance on that is, but just trust me when I say that I am both
relieved and delighted to hear that excellent medical institutions
like Stanford are starting to think about electroacupuncture and how
it can work. That places like Harvard Medical School are starting to
explore this at a mechanistic level. And I do believe that there's an
open-mindedness that starting to emerge for instance, the National
Institutes of Health, not only has an institute for mental health and
cancer research and an eye institute, but now complementary health,
the so-called NCCIH. National Institutes of Complementary Health that
is exploring things like electroacupuncture, meditation, various
supplements and things of those sort. I do think that we're entering a
new realm in which things like pain and pain management will be met
with more openness by all physicians, at least that's my hope. So
please take that into consideration right now. The mechanistic
evidence for laser photobiomodulation is not strong.
One of the major issues or the barriers to that is that most of the
studies that are out there were actually paid for by companies that
build devices for laser photobiomodulation. And so we really need
independent studies funded by federal institutions that have no bias
or financial relationship in order to gain trust in whatever data
happen to emerge. There is a technique that at one time was considered
alternative, but now has a lot of mechanistic science to explain how
it works, and it does indeed work for the treatment of chronic and
also for acute pain. And that treatment is hypnosis in particular
self-hypnosis. Now, my colleague at Stanford in fact, my collaborator,
Dr. David Spiegel, our Associate Chair of Psychiatry has devoted his
professional life to developing hypnosis tools that people can use to
help them sleep better, focus better, stay motivated, et cetera. While
most people hear hypnosis and they think, oh, this is stage hypnosis.
People walking around like chickens are being forced to laugh or fall
asleep on command, et cetera. This is completely different than all
that. This is self hypnosis and there are now dozens, if not more
quality peer reviewed studies published in excellent journals done by
Dr. Spiegel and others at other universities. It really all has to do
with how self-hypnosis can modulate activity of the prefrontal cortex
and related structures like the insula. The prefrontal cortex is
involved in our executive function as it's called, our planning, our
decision making, but also how we interpret context, what the meaning
of a given sensation is.
And that's extremely powerful. I just want to remind everybody that
the currency of the brain and body has not changed in hundreds of
thousands of years. It's always been dopamine, serotonin, glutamate,
GABA, testosterone, estrogen. What's changed are the contingencies,
the events in the world that drive whether or not we get an increase
or decrease in testosterone or estrogen, the events in the world that
dictate whether or not we get an increase or a decrease in dopamine.
Believe me, the events that drove those increases and decreases were
very different even a 100 years ago than they are now. And as we
create new things and societies change, et cetera, they will continue
to exchange information in the same currency, which is dopamine,
serotonin, and all these other neuromodulators and chemicals. Hypnosis
takes advantage of this by allowing an individual, you, if you like to
change the way that you interpret particular events and to actually
experience what would be painful as less painful or not painful. And
that's just the example of pain. Hypnosis is powerful for other
reasons too. It actually can help rewire neural circuits so that you
don't experience as much pain so that you can sleep faster, focus
faster. If this is all sounding very fantastical well, it's supported
by data. The data are that when people do self-hypnosis even brief
self-hypnosis of 10 or 15 minutes, a few times a week, maybe even
returned to that hypnosis by just using a one minute a day hypnosis,
they can achieve significant and often very impressive degrees of pain
relief in chronic pain whether or not that chronic pain arises through
things like fibromyalgia or through other sources. If you want to
check this out, there's a wonderful zero cost resource that's grounded
in this work. It's the app reveri.com. So R-E-V-E-R-I.com. There you
can download a zero cost app for Apple phones or for Android phones.
And there are a variety of different hypnosis scripts. These are
actually self hypnosis scripts, and you'll actually hear Dr. David
Spiegel talking to you. He can teach you about hypnosis and how it
works. There are links to scientific studies that web address that I
gave you before reveri.com. You can see the various studies and the
various write-ups related to those studies and how this all works. And
they're simple protocols. You just click on a tab and you listen to
the self-hypnosis and it will take you into hypnosis. And several of
those hypnosis grips have been shown clinically to relieve certain
patterns of chronic pain. So it's a powerful tool, and I encourage you
not to write off the non-drug non supplement tools as less than
powerful because indeed many people experience tremendous relief from
them. And of course, they also can be combined with drug treatments if
that's right for you or with supplements and things of that sort to
treat pain, if that's right for you. So again, electroacupuncture now
often supported by insurance, not always, but often. Great mechanistic
data starting to emerge. Hypnosis, terrific tool. There's even the
self-hypnosis tool that one can access through the zero cost app
Reveri and lots of great clinical data and scientific mechanistic
data. There are neuro imaging studies showing that different brain
areas are activated in hypnosis the so-called default network, kind of
where your brains is kind of idols and the different circuits that are
active in at rest shift with hypnosis and shift long-term in ways that
positively conserve you. And then these things like laser
photobiomodulation still more or less in that experimental medical
community. I should say, Western Medical Community, not so certain,
but hopefully there will be data soon, and hopefully those data will
point to mechanisms that allow the insurance companies and other sort
of medical bodies to support them if indeed they have a mechanistic
basis. I just want to briefly touch on a common method of pain relief
that speaks to a more general principle of how things like
electroacupuncture, and also some of these new emerging techniques of
kind of like active tissue release and this principle that you hear a
lot about in sports medicine now that when you have pain or injury at
one site, that you should provide pressure above and below that site.
You may have seen this in the Olympics, which is ongoing now where
people will put tape on their body at certain locations oftentimes.
The logic or what they're saying is that this is designed to create
relief in a joint or in a limb that's below the tape, not necessarily
under the tape, but above or below. So for instance, if there's pain
in one shoulder, sometimes we'll put it on the trapezius muscle or
things of that sort. It turns out that there is a basis for this
because of the way that these different nerves run in from the skin
and from the muscles up into the spinal cord and into the brainstem
providing pressure on one nerve pathway can often impact another
pathway. And the simplest and most common example of this is one that
we all do instinctually or intuitively even animals do this. This is
something that in the textbooks is all is called the Gate Theory of
Pain developed by Melzack and Wall kind of classic theory. Basically
we have receptors in our skin, the so-called C fibers, that's just a
name for these little wires that come from a particular class of DRGs
that's very thin that brings about certain kinds of nasal scepter
information. I want to say pain information, but then the pain people
believe are not their pain people. Sometimes they're a pain because
what they tell me is they're on pain receptors okay, nociceptors That
information comes in C fibers and what happens when we injure
something well, provided that we won't damage it worse by touching it,
oftentimes what we will do is we will rub the source of pain or the
location in which we were experiencing pain. And it turns out that's
not an unuseful thing to do. When we rub our skin or an area, or we
provide pressure nearby it, we activate the so-called A fibers, the
bigger wires and neurons that innovate, meaning they jut into that
area of skin. And those A fibers, the ones that respond to mechanical
pressure actually are able to inhibit those C fibers, the ones that
are carrying that so-called pain information. So rubbing an area or
providing pressure above or below an injury actually provides real
pain relief support for the location of that injury or that pain
because of the way that these different patterns or these different
types of neurons interact with one another. And when I say it inhibits
it, I don't mean that it like shouts at it, what it does is it
releases it's literally kind of like vomits up a little bit of a
neurotransmitter called GABA. And GABA is a neurotransmitter that
inhibits it quiets the activity of other neurons. And so it's acting
as kind of an analgesic, if you will, it's acting as its own form of
drug that you make with your body to quiet the activity of these pain
neurons. So rubbing a wound provided it doesn't damage the wound worse
or providing pressure above or below typically it's above a particular
injury can have a real effect in relieving some of the pain of that
injury. And some people have speculated this as through fascia, or
this is through other bodily organs and tissues. And it might be we're
going to do a whole episode on fascia. It's extremely interesting
tissue, but right now it seems that the main source of that pain
relief is through this A fiber inhibition of these C fibers so-called
Melzack and Wall Gate Theory of Pain if you'd like to look it up and
learn about that further.
Now, let's talk about a phenomenon that has long intrigued and
perplexed people for probably thousands of years. And that's redheads.
You may have heard before that redheads have a higher pain threshold
than other individuals. And indeed, that is true. There's now a study
that looked at this mechanistically. There's a gene called the MC1R
gene. And this MC1R gene encodes for a number of different proteins.
Some of those proteins of course are related to the production of
melanin. This is why redheads often not always, but often are very
fair skinned. Sometimes have freckles, not always. And of course have
red hair. Some people are really intense ginger's not psychologically
or emotionally intense perhaps that too, but meaning their hair is
very, very red. Others, it's a lighter red. So of course there's
variation here, but this gene, this MC1R gene is associated with a
pathway that relates to something that I've talked about on this
podcast before during the episode on hunger and feeding and this is
POMC. POMC stands for pro-opiomelanocortin and POMC is cut up. It's
cleaved into different hormones, including one that enhances pain
perception. This is melanocyte stimulating hormone. And another one
that blocks pain beta endorphin. Now, if you listen to the episodes on
testosterone and estrogen and the episodes on hunger and feeding, some
of these molecules will start to ring a bell. Things like melano
stimulating hormone relate to pigmentation of the skin relate to
sexual arousal, et cetera, but it turns out that in red heads, because
of the fact that they have this gene, this MC1R gene, the POMC Pro-
opiomelanocortin, that's cut into different hormones, melanocyte-
stimulating hormones and another one beta endorphin. Beta endorphin
should cue you to the fact that this is in the pain pathway. The
endorphins are endogenously made, meaning made within our body
opioids. They actually make us feel numb in response to certain kinds
of pain. Now, not completely numb, but they numb or reduce our
perception of pain because of the ways in which they are released from
certain brain centers. We'll talk about those brain centers in a
moment. So what's really interesting is that this study showed that
the presence of these hormones is in everybody. We all have
melanocortin 4, we all have beta endorphins. We all have POMC et
cetera, but red heads make more of these endogenous endorphins. And
that's interesting. It allows them to buffer against the pain
response. I have a personal anecdote to share with you about this red
head and heightened levels of pain tolerance phenomenon. Obviously I'm
not a redhead. I don't dye my hair, but my partner for many years was
a red head and still is a red head. She had bright red hair and had
that since childhood. Well, we had the fortunate experience of
becoming friends with Wim Hof and family. They actually came out to
visit us and did a series of seminars in the bay area. This was in
2016, as I recall. And my partner, she had never done an ice bath. She
had never done any kind of real cold water exposure experience before,
but as one particular gathering as is often the case when women's
around, there was an ice bath and a number of people were getting into
this thing. This was actually before a dinner event. And I think for
most people who have never done an ice bath getting in for 30 seconds
or a minute is tolerable, but it takes some effort. It takes some
willpower and take some overcoming that pain barrier 'cause it is a
little bit painful. Not a lot. Some people can stay in longer three
minutes, five minutes without much discomfort. What was incredible is
that without any desire to compete with anybody else, my partner
redhead got into the ice bath and just like sat there for 10 minutes.
In fact, at one point she just kind of turned to me and said, "I don't
really feel pain. I'm not really in pain." And Wim loved this. Wim
thought it was great. He thought it was the most terrific thing in the
world. And he got back in the ice bath and they became fast friends,
and I think they're probably still fast friends. So in any event,
that's an end of one. What we call an anecdata example. Anecdata is
not really a term that we should use too much 'cause it's N of one
anecdotes are just that. They're just anecdotes. But it's been
described many times in various clinics by anesthesiologists, by
observation of coaches, et cetera, that redheads men and women who are
redheads seem to have this higher pain threshold. And it does seem to
be because their body naturally produces ways to counter the pain
response. They produce their own endogenous opioids. Now this of
course should not be taken to mean that redheads can tolerate more
pain and therefore should be subjected to more pain, all it means is
that their threshold for pain on average, not all of them, but on
average is shifted higher than that of other individuals. And it
remains to be determined whether or not other light skin, light haired
individuals also have a heightened level of pain threshold. And I
should mention because I mentioned the ice bath that of course pain
threshold is something that can be built up and provide you do that
safely in ways that aren't damaging your tissues because of course,
pain is a signal that is designed to help you to keep from harming
yourself, but provided that you can do that in a way that's safe and
doesn't damage your tissues, increasing your pain threshold through
the use of things like ice baths is something that really can be done.
It has a lot to do with these contextual or top-down modulations of
the experience. You can tell yourself that this is good for me, or I'm
doing this by choice or whatever it is. You can distract yourself.
There are a huge number of different ways that one could do that. One
of the more interesting ways for which there are actually really good
scientific data come from my colleagues, Sean Mackey's Lab.
And that actually looked at how love and in particular, the experience
of obsessive love could actually counter the pain response, not just
in redheads, but in everybody. So that study I'll just briefly
describe it involved having people come into the laboratory and
experience any one or a number of different painful stimuli, but they
had selectively recruited subjects that were in new relationships for
which there was a high degree of infatuation so much so that the
people couldn't stop thinking about or communicating with that new
partner up to 80% of their waking time, which is a lot. That constant
obsessing about that partner was correlated with. It wasn't causal
necessarily, but was correlated with the ability to sustain higher
levels of pain than people who were in more typical non obsessive
forms of love, longstanding relationships, where there wasn't long
obsessive love rather. And of course in this study, there were a lot
of good control groups. They included a distractor, they included
people obsessing about other things, their pet, et cetera. They
included other forms of love and attachment, but it does seem that
certain patterns of thinking can allow us to buffer ourselves against
the pain response. And that should not be surprising. Certain forms of
thinking are associated with the release of particular neuromodulators
in particular dopamine. And dopamine, it may seem is kind of the thing
that underlies everything, but it's not. Dopamine is a molecule that's
associated with novelty expectation, motivation, and reward. We talked
about this at the beginning of the episode, that it's really the
molecule of expectation and motivation and hope and excitement more
than it's associated with the receival of the reward. Well, dopamine
is coursing throughout the brain at heightened levels and coursing
throughout the body at heightened levels when we fall in love. This
probably has some adaptive mechanism that ensure paired bonding
between people or who knows, maybe it ensured not bonding to multiple
people. Nobody really knows how dopamine functions in terms of pair
bonding, but it is known that when people fall in love, new
relationships create very high levels of dopamine. And that's probably
the mechanistic basis by which these people were able to buffer the
pain response by thinking about their partner or this new relationship
that they're in almost obsessively or obsessively. Now that raises a
deeper question we should always be asking. Yeah, but how, how? Well,
the dopamine system can have powerful effects on the inflammation
system. And it doesn't do this through mysterious ways. It does this
by interacting through the brainstem and some of the neurons that
innervate the spleen and other areas of the body, that deploy cells to
go combat infection, inflammation, and pain. And the ways in which
dopamine can modulate pain, and in this case, this particular study
transform our experience of pain. Maybe even to something that's
pleasureful is not mysterious. It's really through the activation of
brainstem neurons that communicate with areas of our body, that deploy
things like immune cells. So for instance, we have neurons in our
brain stem that can be modulated by the release of dopamine and those
neurons in the brainstem control the release of immune cells from
tissues like the spleen or organs like the spleen. And those immune
cells can then go combat infection. We've heard before that when we're
happy, we're better able to combat infection, deal with pain, deal
with all sorts of things that essentially makes us more resilient. And
that's not because dopamine is some magic molecule, it's because
dopamine affects particular circuits and tells in a very neuro-
biological way in a biochemical way tells those cells and circuits
that conditions are good. Despite the fact that there's pain in the
body conditions are good. You're in love or conditions are good. You
want to be in this experience. Or conditions are good this is for a
greater cause that you're fighting or suffering for some larger
purpose. So all of that has existed largely in the realm of psychology
and even motivational literature in this kind of thing, but there's a
real mechanistic basis for it. Dopamine is a molecule that can bind to
receptor sites on these brain areas. Those brain areas can then
modulate the organs and tissues of the body that can allow us to lean
into challenge. And those challenges can be infection, it can be
physical pain, it can be long bouts of effort that are required of us.
And I think many people have described the feeling of being newly in
love as a heightened level of energy, a capacity do anything. I mean,
the whole concept of a muse is one in which some individual or some
thing either imagined or real enters our life and we can use that as
fuel. And that fuel is chemical fuel and that chemical fuel is
dopamine. And it really does allow for more resilience and can even
transform the experience of pain or what would otherwise be pain into
an experience of pleasure. So, along those lines, let's talk about
pleasure. With all the cells and tissues and machinery related to
pain, you might think that our entire touch system is designed to
allow us to detect pain and to avoid tissue damage and well, a good
percentage of it is devoted to that.
A good percentage of it is also devoted to this thing that we call
pleasure. And that should come as no surprise. Pleasure isn't just
there for our pleasure. It serves adaptive role, and that adaptive
role relates to the fact that every species has a primary goal which
is to make more of itself otherwise it would go extinct. That process
of making more of itself sexual reproduction is closely associated
with the sensation and the perception of pleasure. And it's no
surprise that not only is the highest density of sensory receptors in
and on and around the genitalia, but the process of reproduction
evokes sensations and molecules and perceptions associated with
pleasure. And the currency of pleasure exists in multiple chemical
systems but the primary ones are the dopamine system, which is the
anticipation of pleasure and the work required to achieve the ability
to experience that pleasure, and the serotonin system which is more
closely related to the immediate experience of that pleasure. And from
dopamine and serotonin stem out other hormones and molecules, things
like oxytocin, which are associated with pair bonding. Oxytocin is
more closely associated with the serotonin system biochemically and at
the circuit level meaning the areas of the brain and body that
manufacture a lot of serotonin, usually not always, but usually
contain neurons that also manufacturer and make use of the molecule
oxytocin. Those chemicals together create sensations of warmth, of
well being, of safety. The dopamine molecule is more closely
associated with hormones like testosterone and other molecules
involved with pursuit and further effort in order to get more of
whatever could potentially cause more release of dopamine. So this is
a very broad strokes, no pun intended description of the pleasure
system. There are of course, other molecules as well.
One in particular that's very interesting is something called PEA.
PEA, it stands for Phenethylamine sometimes also referred to as
Phenethylamine depending on who you are and where you live, how you
pronounce it doesn't really matter. PEA is a molecule, which is
incredibly potent at augmenting or increasing the activity of certain
cells and neural circuits that relate to the pleasure system. PEA has
purportedly been thought to be released in response to ingestion of
things like certain forms of dark chocolate. Some people take it in
supplement form. It's a bit of a stimulant, but it also seems to
heighten the perception of pleasure in response to a particular amount
of dopamine and or serotonin. So for instance, in a kind of a
arbitrary experiment and units type example, if a given experience
evokes a particular amount of serotonin and dopamine and gives rise to
a subjective experience of pleasure of say level three out of 10, the
ingestion of PEA prior to that experience can increase the rating of
that experience as more pleasureful. Maybe a four or a five, or even a
six. And PEA is known to be present in or I should say it's releases
stimulated by a number of different compounds, such as dark chocolate,
certain things like aspartame and certain people can actually increase
the amount of PEA released. Some of these glutamate related molecules
like aspartame or things are in the glutamate pathway can increase PEA
release. And then some people will actually take PEA in supplement
form for its mild stimulant properties as well as for increasing the
perception of, or the ability to experience pleasure. It's not a
sledgehammer. It's not like dopamine itself. People that take things
like Mucuna pruriens, L-DOPA or drugs of abuse, which I certainly
don't recommend things like cocaine or amphetamine experience
tremendous increases in dopamine, not so much increases in serotonin.
Some people will take serotonin in precursor form like 5HTP or
serotonin itself, or they'll take the amino acid precursor like
tryptophan. I'm not saying these things as recommendations for
increasingly one sense of pleasure, I'm describing them because of
what they do generally falls into two categories. The first category
is to raise the foundation, what we call the tonic level of dopamine
and serotonin. So if levels of serotonin and dopamine are too low, it
becomes almost impossible to experience pleasure. There's a so-called
anhedonia. This is also described as depression. Although it needn't
be long-term depression. So certain drugs like antidepressants like
Wellbutrin Bupropion as it's commonly called or the so-called SSRI,
the serotonin selective re-uptake inhibitors like Prozac, Zoloft and
similar will increase dopamine and serotonin respectively. They're not
increasing the peaks in those molecules. What we call the acute
release of those molecules, what they're doing is they're raising the
overall levels of those molecules. They're raising the sort of
foundation or the tide if you will, think about it as your mood or
your pleasure rather is like a boat, and if it's on the shore and it
can't get out to sea, unless that tide is high enough, that's kind of
the way to think about these tonic levels of dopamine and serotonin.
Now, most of us fortunately, do not have problems with our baseline or
autonic levels of dopamine and serotonin release. Things like PEA in
that case will cause a slight increase in that tide and make the
ability of certain experiences to increase dopamine further more
available.
What we call this in neurosciences so-called gain control. I can kind
of turn up the volume, bring us closer to the threshold to activate
certain circuits. And this is really what we mean when we say a
neuromodulator, okay? This is why when you are very happy about
something, let's say you're out with your friends. You're really
excited. Maybe depending on where you live and what's going on in your
area of the world right now, like I have a niece and she's been locked
up in quarantine for a long time recently because it was deemed safe.
She got to go to summer camp. I have never seen that kid so happy to
spend with her friends. She was so excited and it was really amazing
to see how excited she was. Her baseline levels of dopamine were
clearly up so much so that when she saw her friends, she literally
started squealing. They were squealing, she was squealing. Everyone
was squealing. I wasn't squealing. I would admit it if I was
squealing. I wasn't squealing, but it was such a delight to see and
I'm sure that made my dopamine levels go up, which was, she was just
so excited such that anything and everything felt like an exciting
stimulus. This is pleasure, right? And I don't want to write off the
experience from an neuro-biological reductionist standpoint, quite the
opposite. It's really beautiful to see again this principle that
different experiences and the experience of pleasure from different
things. Seeing your friends for the first time, summer camp for a kid,
whatever it might happen to be use the same currency, dopamine use the
same currency serotonin. And this is a principle that I hope in
listening to this podcast and even some of it's repetitive features
from one episode to the next. I'm hoping that those will start to
embed in your mind that the brain and body use these common currencies
for different experiences. So yes, if your dopamine and serotonin, or
I should say if your dopamine and or serotonin levels are too low, it
will be very hard to achieve pleasure to experience physical pleasure
or emotional pleasure of any kind. That's why treatments of the sort
that I described a minute ago might be right for you. Obviously we
can't determine if they're right for you. It's also why they have side
effects. If you artificially increase these molecules they're
associated with pleasure, oftentimes you get a lack of motivation to
go seek things like food. People don't get much interest in food
'cause why should they if their serotonin levels are already up.
Again, there's a ton of individual variation. I don't want to say that
these antidepressants are always bad. Sometimes they've saved lives.
They've saved millions of lives. Sometimes people have side effects
that make them not the right choice. So it has to be determined for
the individual. Things like PEA or a more subtle effect. I should
mention PEA supplementation is something that a number of people use
but it's very short-lived. Because of the half-life of this molecule
was very brief, the effect only lasts about 20 minutes or so. Things
like L-dopa, Mucuna pruriens lead to longer baseline increases in
dopamine. But remember, any time you raise a baseline, you reduce the
so-called signal to noise. What it means is if you're riding around at
really high dopamine, at first, everything will start to seem exciting
like my niece and seeing her friends for the first time. Everything's
exciting. But then what will happen is when your dopamine levels
return to more normal levels, it will take a much greater dopamine
increase of much bigger event, more novel, more exciting in order to
achieve the sense that what you're experiencing is pleasureful. And
this is because of the relationship between pleasure and pain. Now, in
a future episode we are going to go deep into this relationship
between pleasure and pain, but just briefly as a precursor to that and
because it's relevant to the conversation that we've been having, you
might want to be wary of any experience, any experience, no matter how
it arrives, chemical, physical, emotional, or some combination, you
might want to be wary of letting your dopamine go too high and
certainly you want to be wary of it going too low.
Because of the way that these circuits adjust. Basically every time
that the pleasure system is kicked in in high gear, an absolutely
spectacular event, you cannot be more ecstatic. There is a mirror
symmetric activation of the pain system. And this might seem like an
evil curse of biology, but it's not. This is actually a way to protect
this whole system of reward and motivation that I talked about at the
beginning of the episode. It might sound great to just ingest
substances or engage in behaviors where it's just dopamine, dopamine,
dopamine, and just constantly be motivated, but the system will
eventually crash. And so what happens is when you have a big increase
in dopamine, you also will get a big increase in the circuits that
underlie our sense of disappointment and re adjusting the balance. And
with repeated exposure to high levels of dopamine, not naturally
occurring wonderful events, but really high chemically induced peaks
in dopamine, high magnitude, chemically induced peaks in dopamine.
What happens is those peaks in dopamine start to go down and down and
down in response to the same, what ought to be incredible experience.
We start to what's called habituate or attenuate, and yet the pain
increases in size. And this has a preservative function in keeping us
safe, believe it or not. But what I just described is actually the
basis of most if not all, forms of addiction something that we will
deal with in a future episode in depth. So what should you think about
all? How should you think about pleasure and how should you think
about pain? What is too much pleasure?
Well, that's going to differ from person to person, but to the extent
that one can access pleasure repeatedly over time, ideally without
chemical augmentation, certainly not excessive chemical augmentation,
that means that this pleasure system is tuned up well and can continue
to experience pleasure. However, if you find yourself engaging in the
same behavior over and over again, but achieving less and less
pleasure from it, chances are you want to adjust down how often you
engage in that behavior. And or adjust down your expectation of reward
every time you engage in that behavior. What do I mean by that? Well,
at the beginning of the episode I talked about how dopamine will allow
us to get into bouts of hard work. We will work very hard to pursue a
reward, and that's really what dopamine does. And then when the reward
comes that doesn't increase our dopamine. In fact, our dopamine levels
go down. One of the key things that we can all do to adjust our
ability to experience pleasure is to engage in that intermittent
reward schedule. You can either adjust down the peak in dopamine,
meaning not let yourself ever get too happy, but that's no fun, right?
Life is about occasionally achieving or experiencing ecstasy, but
every once in a while, remove the reward. And of course, I don't mean
ecstasy the drug that's a separate matter. The MDMA trials are a
separate matter. Very interesting, I want to be clear. I meant
psychological and physical ecstasy of the natural sort. I've immense
interest in what's going on in the MDMA trials, but just for clarity
purposes, that's a separate topic that we will cover in an episode
very soon. So how do you adjust this dopamine system? Well, every once
in a while at random, not in a predictable way, you remove the reward
and that will keep you and your dopamine system tune up in the proper
ways. The gain of the dopamine system, as we say, will be adjusted so
that you can continue to experience dopamine and serotonin when you
actually get the reward. This can be translated into a huge number of
different domains, but I want to give some examples because I'm sure
that many of you are asking, wait, what does this actually mean? Okay,
let's say you're a student, or this could be a student in academia, or
this could be a student of a physical practice. Every once in a while
when you do something really well, maybe that's even just showing up
to the practice rather than pat yourself on the back, just tell
yourself yeah, that's the minimum that's expected of me. When
everyone's excited about something that you're doing, maybe you're
excited about it, try and adjust down your excitement a little bit. I
know this might seem counterintuitive, but you're preserving the
ability to experience excitement in a variety of contexts. Let's say
you get a big monetary award. Well, that's great. I'm happy for you.
And that's wonderful. However, you should be a little bit wary if you
care about your dopamine system and you care about your ability to get
subsequent monetary rewards, excuse me, awards rewards doesn't matter
which through effort, if you want to be able to maintain the ability
to exert effort, well, then you probably wouldn't want to run out and
immediately buy something with that monetary reward. In other words,
you wouldn't want to layer on more dopamine release, okay? You might,
but you might not. You might skip it. What you'll find then is that
your motivation is essentially infinite. This is what I described at
the beginning of the episode. And again, it's because dopamine is this
currency. It's like these days you hear a lot about Bitcoin, and
Ethereum, and Dogecoin, and USs dollars, and Euros and other stuff.
But the currency that you use in your body doesn't matter what
external currency those are. In fact, as you watch the value of
different currencies go up whether or not it's cryptocurrency or
standard currency, the value is actually reflective of the dopamine
that exists inside of people. So all the excitement about a particular
currency crypto or otherwise is really just dopamine. That's the
currency that we all use. And there's no negotiating that. That's just
the way that we're built. Now, to give yet other examples. Let's say
you're teaching other people how to do something and they do something
exceptionally well. If you reward them every single time. and in
particular, if you reward them with something that's even greater than
the experience of what they did. So let's say kids win a soccer game
and they're ecstatic. They're jumping all over the place. They're
super excited and you reward them with an even bigger experience, a
celebration, you are actually inhibiting their ability to perform the
same set of activities that led them to the win if, and I really want
to underscore if you reward them every time. Of course we should
reward kids and each other and ourselves for our accomplishments, but
you don't want to do it every time. And sure there will be some
disappointment from suddenly removing the reward that you expected,
but that's exactly the point. That's what keeps these circuits tuned
up properly.
Now there's the other form of pleasure, which is the more immediate
visceral or sensory experience of pleasure. This is distinct from
goals and goal-directed behavior. I'm talking about the immediate
experience. This is more of the serotonergic system. There are other
systems involved too, but this is also the system that draws out those
endogenous opioids from a particular structure. We have a structure in
the back of our brain called PAG P-A-G, it's the periaqueductal gray
area. Very interesting brain area that is associated with pain, but
also with pleasure because under certain conditions, it deploys
endogenous opioids and gives us a kind of blissed out feeling. This is
not like the opioids of the opioid epidemic sort that people take and
unfortunately have led to tremendous amounts of suffering and abuse.
These are endogenously released opioids. These are the kinds of
opioids that come out from long distance bouts of physical exercise
and running. These are the opioids that are deployed in response to
giving birth and overcoming the tremendous pain of childbirth. So PAG
is very contextual and there are few types of stimuli or I should say
events in life... I'm really showing my nerdy side. There are a few
types of stimulate, I'm talking about experiences that evoke
endogenous opioid release from PAG. One is sexual activity. Sexual
activity can increase pain threshold. And here I am not suggesting or
getting involved in anyone's particular proclivities or personal
experiences. You're welcome to editorialize this however you like,
however, what I'm talking about are animal data and yes, human data as
well, that show that pain thresholds are increased anytime PAG is
activated because of the release of these endogenous opioids. There's
also the immediate experience of whether or not a particular form of
touch is pleasureful or not. And there there's some very interesting
biology that relates to really how those little wires from those DRGs
innovate our skin.
Work studies I should say done by David Ginty's lab at Harvard Medical
School, the Ginty lab has spent years working on the somata sensory
system, the touch system has identified a particular category of
neurons that innervate the skin and then those neurons of course send
that information up to the brain too. And they actually respond to
direction of touch. Now, some of you might be more sensitive to this
than others, but it turns out that certain hairs like to be deflected
one way versus another. Whether you like cats or not, you can do this
experiment. You can pet a cat in the direction that they're fur lies.
So it lies down in a particular direction. You'll notice that there's
actually a gene that dictates that the hairs lie down in a particular
direction. And if you pet them in a way that's co-operating with that
direction. So not pushing the hairs up, but rather stroking the hairs
on the back of the cat. Well, you'll notice as they often like that.
Not all cats some cats are pretty grouchy, but if you stroke their
hair, they will often per, they'll often push into you. If you were to
stroke their hair in the opposite direction, pushing the hairs up
against the direction that they want to lie down, cats do not like
that. And it turns out that people don't like that either. Some people
do like to have their hair pushed in a direction against the direction
in which it wants to lay down. But there is more typically response a
feeling like it's pleasureful for instance, when someone brushes or
combs their hair in the direction that it wants to lay down. And
that's because the way in which these neurons, they innovate these
hairs sends information up to the brain bifurcates actually, it splits
into brain centers that evoke a sense of pleasure or a sense of not
pleasure. It's not necessarily pain. So you might find that certain
people are very particular. They like to be touched in a certain way,
but not others. You might be one of those people. And areas of our
skin that have high density of receptors are very, very sensitive in a
real way, in a real sense of the word to patterns of touch and whether
or not a touch is too firm or too light. And that will be modulated by
overall levels of arousal. And when I talk about arousal, what I'm
talking about is how alert or how sleep we are. It is impossible to
experience pain when we are deep in sleep. I don't mean sleeping like
of the typical night's sort. I mean, of the anesthesia sort. That's
the purpose of anesthesia to bring the brain and body into a deep
plane of rest, very deep in fact, and it's very hard if not impossible
to achieve or experience pleasure when we are in a very low state of
arousal as well. When we are in heightened states of arousal, we can
achieve pain, we can experience pain and we can experience pleasure.
And under those heightened states of arousal, we are more sensitive.
Literally the passage of electrical signals from those locations on
the body that have heightened degrees or higher degrees, I should say
of receptors, use your imagination. They include the lips, the face,
the feet, and the genitals and nearby areas, literally nearby areas.
Under conditions of higher arousal two things happen, the ability to
achieve or experience pleasure at those locations goes up and our
tolerance and our threshold for pain also goes up. So the principle
here is that as our levels of arousal, that foundation of arousal goes
up or down, so too goes up and down our ability to achieve pleasure
and pain. And so these two extremes of being deep within anesthesia or
another extreme as asleep or an heightened levels of arousal, our
ability to achieve pleasure and pain are going to scale according to
those. And this is why, and I'm certainly not suggesting this, but
this is why some people will take stimulants or drugs of abuse that
increase arousal in order to achieve pleasure of other kinds. The
problem is is that those drugs in particular are things like cocaine
and methamphetamine and amphetamine become their own form of
reinforcement so much so that the person doesn't seek out any other
form of excitement or arousal. So today we weren't talking about
addiction.
We weren't necessarily talking about motivation, but we touched on
those topics as sort of a precursor of what's to come. We talked about
the pathways in the skin and in the brain and elsewhere in the body
that control our sense of pleasure and pain. We described a number of
different tools ranging from hypnosis to different supplements, to
electroacupuncture and various other tools that one could use to
modulate your sense of pleasure or pain. And of course, in thinking
about pleasure, we have to think about the dopamine system and the
serotonin system and some of the related chemical systems. I realized
that today's podcast had a lot of scientific details. We've
timestamped everything for you so that you don't have to digest it all
at once of course I don't expect that everyone would be able to
understand all these details all at once. What's more important really
is to understand the general principles of how something like pleasure
and pain work. How they interact, and the very seldom systems within
the brain and body that allow them to occur and that modulator or
change their ability to occur. And of course your subjective
experience of pleasure or pain. So I do hope that this was on hole
more pleasureful than painful for you. If you're enjoying this podcast
and you're learning from it, and you'd like to support us, you can do
that in a number of different ways. Some of which are totally cost-
free. The first one is please subscribe to the YouTube channel. That
really helps us. In addition, you can leave us comments and
suggestions for future podcast episodes on the YouTube channel. You
can also subscribe on Apple and or Spotify or all three that would
really help us. And on Apple, you can leave us up to a five star
review and leave us feedback. There are other ways to support the
podcast as well. We have a Patreon that's patreon.com/andrewhuberman.
And there you can support the podcast at any level that you like. In
addition, please check out our sponsors that we mentioned at the
beginning of the podcast. We only work with sponsors and brands that
we absolutely love their products and that we wholeheartedly endorse,
and that we use ourselves. In addition, we've partnered with Thorne.
Thorne as a supplement company, and we've partnered with them because
they have the highest levels of stringency in terms of the quality of
the ingredients they use and the quantity of the ingredients they use.
By quantity I mean that unlike a lot of supplement companies out
there, the amounts that are listed on the bottle are absolutely what
you find in those capsules and tablets in the bottle. If you go to
Thorne that's T-H-O-R-N-E/U/Huberman, you can see the supplements that
I take and you can get 20% off any of those supplements as well as any
of the other supplements that Thorne makes. Just go into the Thorne
site through that portal, thorne/u/huberman and even if you navigate
off from that location in the site, you'll get 20% off any of the
items that you might select from other locations within the Thorne
site. If you're not already following us on Instagram, it's Huberman
Lab at Instagram, and there I do various tutorials about neuroscience,
offer neuroscience related tools all backed by science. And last but
not least, I thank you for your time and attention and thank you for
your interest in science. [bright upbeat music]
Note: The description of the dorsal root ganglia (DRGs) was
intentionally simplified and does not include mention of dorsal horn
spinal relay neurons, etc.. For an excellent full text review of this
anatomy and circuits for touch sensing, please see:
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3811145/
hubermanlab #pain #pleasure #dopamine #motivation
Thank you to our sponsors:
* InsideTracker - https://www.insidetracker.com/huberman
* Helix Sleep - https://www.helixsleep.com/huberman
* Athletic Greens - https://www.athleticgreens.com/huberman
Our Patreon page:
* https://www.patreon.com/andrewhuberman
Supplements from Thorne:
* http://www.thorne.com/u/huberman
Social:
* Instagram - https://www.instagram.com/hubermanlab
* Twitter - https://twitter.com/hubermanlab
* Facebook - https://www.facebook.com/hubermanlab
Website - https://hubermanlab.com
Join the Neural Network - https://hubermanlab.com/neural-network
Links:
* Instagram discussion with Dr. Sean Mackey, MD, PhD - https://www.instagram.com/p/CMVq0X8Bk1D/
* Agmatine study - https://bit.ly/3CtTwRn
* Mechanistic basis of acupuncture - https://bit.ly/2VHi0pz
Please note that The Huberman Lab Podcast is distinct from Dr.
Huberman's teaching and research roles at Stanford University School
of Medicine. The information provided in this show is not medical
advice, nor should it be taken or applied as a replacement for medical
advice. The Huberman Lab Podcast, its employees, guests and affiliates
assume no liability for the application of the information discussed.
Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac - https://www.blabacphoto.com